2002
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.28.5.983
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Aggregation in memory of episodic influences on rule-guided decisions.

Abstract: The authors confirmed E. Z. Rothkopf and M. L. Dashen's (1995) finding that specific problem context, such as thematic surface features, forms associative connections with deep problem features and thus speeds particular decisions (particularization). In 5 experiments, using a 3-bit decision task and pre-memorized decision rules, the authors found that the ability of a situational context to reinstate was decreased by its replacement by another modal surface context. Context reinstatement, as measured by decis… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2010
2010

Publication Types

Select...
3

Relationship

1
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These include, for example, content structure and redundancy (Bryan and Harter 1899;Anderson 1983), distinctions between nominal stimulation and internal representations (Fechner 1860, Hull 1943Rothkopf 1957;Underwood 1963), the consequences of active engagement (Gates 1917;Hovland et al 1949), techniques to foster attention and engagement (Rothkopf and Bloom 1970;Hyde and Jenkins 1973;Scardamalia et al 1984), the interval between instructive events (e.g., Ebbinghaus 1885; Smith and Rothkopf 1984;Dempster 1996;Pashler and Bayliss 1991;Pashler et al 2003), evocation schedules (Bjork 1988), sequence of presentation in induction (e.g., Detambel and Stolurow 1956;Rothkopf 1958), the role of surface context (Ross 1984;Rothkopf et al 2002), as well as a number of others. The literature is very large, and its interpretation and use for practical applications in teaching designs allows for many possibilities.…”
Section: Goals Of Instructional Researchmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…These include, for example, content structure and redundancy (Bryan and Harter 1899;Anderson 1983), distinctions between nominal stimulation and internal representations (Fechner 1860, Hull 1943Rothkopf 1957;Underwood 1963), the consequences of active engagement (Gates 1917;Hovland et al 1949), techniques to foster attention and engagement (Rothkopf and Bloom 1970;Hyde and Jenkins 1973;Scardamalia et al 1984), the interval between instructive events (e.g., Ebbinghaus 1885; Smith and Rothkopf 1984;Dempster 1996;Pashler and Bayliss 1991;Pashler et al 2003), evocation schedules (Bjork 1988), sequence of presentation in induction (e.g., Detambel and Stolurow 1956;Rothkopf 1958), the role of surface context (Ross 1984;Rothkopf et al 2002), as well as a number of others. The literature is very large, and its interpretation and use for practical applications in teaching designs allows for many possibilities.…”
Section: Goals Of Instructional Researchmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…They assume that practice allows trainees to fine-tune decision rules and hence that performance will improve in situations where prior examples are not available (Erickson & Kruschke, 1998). However, they also assume that when prior examples are available, people will continue to use them, even if they have access to a well-learned rule (Logan, 1988;Rothkopf, Dashen, & Teft, 2002;Vokey & Brooks, 1992;Whittlesea et al, 1994).…”
Section: Training Design and Structurementioning
confidence: 99%