2009
DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2009.04.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Agreement attraction in comprehension: Representations and processes

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

85
735
19
2

Year Published

2012
2012
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 393 publications
(841 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
85
735
19
2
Order By: Relevance
“…This characterization also seems satisfactory to explain the failure of otro as a retrieval cue; there is no entry in memory that otro picks out-except in the Incorrect Attractor-different case, where vestido occurs prior to otro. This condition may induce a local attraction between cue and illicit antecedent that does not occur when there is a matching and also licit item in memory, although our results do not bear evidence for this hypothesis (but see Wagers et al, 2009). Cue-diagnosticity-based attraction thus offers an explanation for the difference between the two Correct conditions-if the attractor noun does not match the features of the antecedent, then local mismatch between the attractor and the correct determiner also impacts ongoing neural processing-as well as for the reduction of the sustained negativity for the Incorrect condition in the presence of a local cue-matching attractor.…”
Section: Cue-based Retrieval and Cue-diagnosticity In Sentence Comprecontrasting
confidence: 80%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This characterization also seems satisfactory to explain the failure of otro as a retrieval cue; there is no entry in memory that otro picks out-except in the Incorrect Attractor-different case, where vestido occurs prior to otro. This condition may induce a local attraction between cue and illicit antecedent that does not occur when there is a matching and also licit item in memory, although our results do not bear evidence for this hypothesis (but see Wagers et al, 2009). Cue-diagnosticity-based attraction thus offers an explanation for the difference between the two Correct conditions-if the attractor noun does not match the features of the antecedent, then local mismatch between the attractor and the correct determiner also impacts ongoing neural processing-as well as for the reduction of the sustained negativity for the Incorrect condition in the presence of a local cue-matching attractor.…”
Section: Cue-based Retrieval and Cue-diagnosticity In Sentence Comprecontrasting
confidence: 80%
“…This pattern seems related in spirit to instances of so-called "local coherence," where local constraints can sometimes outweigh global ones, producing "illusions" of grammaticality Tabor et al, 2004). A similar phenomenon of attraction errors during subject-verb agreement production has been welldocumented in production (e.g., Bock and Miller, 1991;Staub, 2009) and in comprehension in certain cases (Pearlmutter et al, 1999;Wagers et al, 2009). Anaphor resolution has also shown vulnerability to interference that could be attributed to differences in cue-diagnosticity (Badecker and Straub, 2002;Sturt, 2003).…”
Section: Cue-based Retrieval and Cue-diagnosticity In Sentence Comprementioning
confidence: 82%
“…Recent research has also highlighted the occasionally errorprone nature of agreement in comprehension, and specifically shown that agreement processing in comprehension can also be somewhat fragile and show gradient effects of grammaticality when a subject NP contains potentially ambiguous or misleading cues to the grammatical number of the subject (e.g., Tanner, Nicol, & Brehm, 2014;Wagers, Lau, & Phillips, 2009). In light of this, some important research goals are to better understand (1) the range of cues used in the processing of agreement dependencies (e.g., those that modulate error rates and sensitivity to agreement relations), and (2) the potential convergence or divergence in the cues and mechanisms used during language production and comprehension.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The regions of interest consisted of the pronoun (R4) and the three words following it (R5-7), because self-paced reading effects typically spill over after the critical word (Wagers et al, 2009;Parker & Phillips, 2016;Lago, Shalom, Sigman, Lau, & Phillips, 2015). To avoid analyzing each of the three spillover regions separately, which could increase the likelihood of Type 1 errors (von der Malsburg & Angele, 2016), the three regions were averaged on a trial-by-trial basis.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Speeded acceptability judgments provide a time-sensitive measure that has been shown to reliably mirror processing effects by requiring participants to rely on their working memory to construct a representation of the sentence and by restricting the amount of time that they have to reflect on their acceptability intuitions (Drenhaus, Saddy, & Frisch, 2005;Wagers, Lau, & Phillips, 2009;Parker & Phillips, 2016).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%