1993
DOI: 10.1016/0272-7358(93)90031-g
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Alert insomniacs: Are they really sleep deprived?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
30
0
1

Year Published

1995
1995
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 68 publications
(38 citation statements)
references
References 87 publications
7
30
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Self-reported insomnia and Epworth Sleepiness were both related to the self-reported attention and memory measures, but not to the behavioral attention measures. The effects for the self-report measures are consistent with reports that people with insomnia tend to overestimate the impact of sleep loss on cognitive deficits (Chambers & Keller, 1993;Harvey, 2002) and the lack of effects for the behavioral measures are consistent with frequent findings of no differences in neuropsychological functioning between people with insomnia and good-sleepers (Semler & Harvey, 2005). The lack of association of insomnia and Epworth Sleepiness scores with behavioral measures may have also arisen because subjects were able to select the time of participating and may have selected periods of self-assessed optimal functioning.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…Self-reported insomnia and Epworth Sleepiness were both related to the self-reported attention and memory measures, but not to the behavioral attention measures. The effects for the self-report measures are consistent with reports that people with insomnia tend to overestimate the impact of sleep loss on cognitive deficits (Chambers & Keller, 1993;Harvey, 2002) and the lack of effects for the behavioral measures are consistent with frequent findings of no differences in neuropsychological functioning between people with insomnia and good-sleepers (Semler & Harvey, 2005). The lack of association of insomnia and Epworth Sleepiness scores with behavioral measures may have also arisen because subjects were able to select the time of participating and may have selected periods of self-assessed optimal functioning.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…The dissociation between fatigue and sleepiness ratings has been highlighted previously, with the former more frequently reported in insomnia. 71,72 Controlling for OSA severity, overweight/obese participants were more likely to belong to the IRD than the MI symptom cluster profile. Our findings report on cross-sectional data, and thus preclude inference about causal relationships between insomnia-related distress and obesity; however, these results might suggest avenues for further investigation.…”
Section: "Insomnia-related Distress" Symptom Cluster Profilementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Tagalakis, Amsel, Brender, & Creti, 1998). When older subjects are divided into good and poor sleeper categories, some longitudinal studies have even suggested that sleep quality can improve over time (Libman et al, 1998;Mellinger et al, 1985; MendeIson, 1995; Monjan & Foley, 1995;Morgan et al, 1989 & Libman, 1998;Chambers & Keller, 1993;Hauri & Fisher, 1986;Zarnmit, 1988).…”
Section: Definition Of Insomniamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Reports such as these have lead some researchers to conclude that the daytime fatigue and sleepiness commonly attributed to poor sleep or to sleep deprivation by patients with insomnia may be more strongly associated with psychologicai variables and personality characteristics (e.g., a general tendency to cornplain of somatic distress) than to nocturnal sleep quaiity (e.g., Chambers & Keller, 1993). Another possible explanation is that subjectively experienced daytime performance and behavioral aspects of these constnicts may, in fact, not be controlled by the same physiological mechanisms.…”
Section: Definition Of Insomniamentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation