The scale illusion (Deutsch, 1975) shows the importance of frequency range in the perceptual organization of a sequence of notes. This paper includes three experiments on the scale illusion. Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrated that if the structure of the pattern of notes used in the original scale illusion study is altered slightly, by adding or subtracting a pair of notes from the ends ofthe sequence, there is a significant decrease in the rate of frequency-based responses, suggesting a weaker illusion. Experiment 3 investigated two features of the note patterns that may have led to this change. Specifically, it asked whether the decrease in the strength of the illusion is due to (1) the nature of the notes at the extremes of the frequency range and/or (2) the nature of the notes at the crossing point of the two scales. While both sources were found to affect the strength of the scale illusion, the former had a greater influence.Frequency range is known to provide a reliable basis for the perceptual organization of complex tonal streams (Bregman & Campbell, 1971) and to provide a strong cue that makes the task of recognizing interleaved melodies easier (Dowling, 1968(Dowling, , 1973Hartmann & Johnson, 1991). Organizing by frequency range is conceptually equivalent to organizing according to the gestalt principle of proximity. An alternative basis for the organization of notes is provided by their "tonal trajectory" (cf. Tougas & Bregman, 1987, 1990 as manifested in such ascending or descending structures as major, minor, and chromatic scales. Such an organization represents a form of good continuation.When put in competition, the influence of a good continuation organization does not approach the strength of proximity. Bregman (1987, 1990) presented pure-tone stimuli that could be organized equally well on the basis of proximity (frequency range) or good continuation (a pattern of tones that progressively ascended or descended). Listeners unwaveringly reported proximitybased perceptions. This preference occurred even when emphasis on the tonal trajectory was given by clear ascending and descending passages prior to the critical sequence.A third basis for organizing musical notes is by their spatial location. For example, one could group by ear of input in a dichotic listening experiment, that is, accord- ing to laterality. Laterality, like good continuation, is apparently less persuasive than proximity. Deutsch (1974) showed this in an effect called the "octave illusion." She presented a dichotic sequence of notes an octave apart, with the notes in the two octaves repeatedly switching ears. When asked what was perceived in each ear, listeners did not report the alternating high and low notes'that were truly presented. Instead, they reported the high notes lateralized in one ear and the low notes in the other.Finally, proximity has been found to be the preferred organization when put in competition with both good continuation and laterality. This is the essence of an effect called the scale illusion (Butler, 19...