2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2012.05.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Amplitude effects of medio-lateral mechanical and visual perturbations on gait

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

7
12
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
7
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…3B, Table 3) dynamic instability of the ML COM movements of all body segments. This indicates that visual and walking surface perturbations significantly affect whole body stability, as previously shown (McAndrew et al, 2011; Sinitski et al, 2012; Terry et al, 2012). Importantly, the present findings extend these prior results to other body segments.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 82%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…3B, Table 3) dynamic instability of the ML COM movements of all body segments. This indicates that visual and walking surface perturbations significantly affect whole body stability, as previously shown (McAndrew et al, 2011; Sinitski et al, 2012; Terry et al, 2012). Importantly, the present findings extend these prior results to other body segments.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 82%
“…2) with incommensurate frequencies (McAndrew et al, 2010; Beurskens et al, 2014): A(t)=AW[1.0sin(0.16x2πt)+0.8sin(0.21x2πt)+1.4sin(0.24x2πt)+0.5sin(0.49x2πt)], where A(t) was the perturbation amplitude in meters, A w was a weighting factor in meters and t was time in seconds. Perturbation magnitudes were set to A w =0.05 for PLAT and to A w =0.50 for VIS to generate approximately comparable responses for each perturbation type (Terry et al, 2012). Throughout each trial, participants’ head orientation was monitored visually to ensure they were looking at the horizon directly in front of them.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In adults, visual deprivation during walking induces negative changes in the spatiotemporal patterns of gait, such as a reduction in walking speed and step length and an increase in double support duration [10][11][12] . The same gait adaptations are observed in children walking in the dark 13 , in subjects walking in a virtual reality environment which induces conflicts to the visual system 14,15 , and in subjects with visual problems 12,16,17 . Apparently, these patterns are adopted as a strategy to keep the COM close to the BOS, maintaining a careful gait as seen when walking in situations with an increased risk of slipping 7 or in elderly populations with a history of falls 18,19 .…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 60%
“…In the case of adaptation to slower walking speeds, step length should decrease to prevent the loss of stability and risk of falls 7 . Visual perturbations do not impose changes during walking, but rather cause motor adaptations characterized by relatively individual responses to permit gait control 15 . During visual deprivation, these alterations in gait correction are seen in situations of overcoming obstacles 22 and in the control of gait termination 25 .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%