2009
DOI: 10.1901/jaba.2009.42-123
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

An Evaluation of the Relative Efficacy of and Children's Preferences for Teaching Strategies That Differ in Amount of Teacher Directedness

Abstract: The manner in which teachers mediate children's learning varies across early childhood classrooms. In this study, we used a multielement design to evaluate the efficacy of three commonly implemented strategies that varied in teacher directedness for teaching color- and object-name relations. Strategy 1 consisted of brief exposure to the target relations followed by an exclusively child-led play period in which correct responses were praised. Strategy 2 was similar except that teachers prompted the children to … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
45
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 48 publications
(49 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
4
45
0
Order By: Relevance
“…As Heal et al (2009) noted, preference outcomes may not be influenced primarily by the reinforcing features of the preferred context (e.g., relative immediacy, quality, and magnitude of reinforcement; less exposure to extinction), but may instead be the result of a dynamic interaction between the reinforcing features of the preferred context and the aversive features of the nonpreferred contexts (e.g., delay to reinforcement). Cumulative initial-link selections (second row) during the preference assessments for Ed and Dee across sessions.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 98%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…As Heal et al (2009) noted, preference outcomes may not be influenced primarily by the reinforcing features of the preferred context (e.g., relative immediacy, quality, and magnitude of reinforcement; less exposure to extinction), but may instead be the result of a dynamic interaction between the reinforcing features of the preferred context and the aversive features of the nonpreferred contexts (e.g., delay to reinforcement). Cumulative initial-link selections (second row) during the preference assessments for Ed and Dee across sessions.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…The procedure for paired-card presentations was similar to that described by Fisher et al (1992) with the exception that every color-card selection resulted in brief social praise (i.e., nondifferential consequences were arranged in this assessment; Heal, Hanley, & Layer, 2009). The procedure for paired-card presentations was similar to that described by Fisher et al (1992) with the exception that every color-card selection resulted in brief social praise (i.e., nondifferential consequences were arranged in this assessment; Heal, Hanley, & Layer, 2009).…”
Section: Color Preference Assessmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children's values with respect to teaching procedures have also been assessed in a similar manner (Heal, & Hanley, 2007;Heal, Hanley, & Layer, 2009). In Heal et al, the preferences of 4 children of typical development for teaching strategies that varied in the amount of teacher directedness were evaluated.…”
Section: Single-person Applicationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although there are multiple comparative studies examining different instructional components, few of these studies focused on teaching play targets. Recent research has examined child preferences for instruction and found that (a) preference is not always related to efficacy and (b) preferences can be modifi ed using observation or changes to reinforcement contingencies (Hanley, Tiger, & Ingvarsson, 2009 ;Heal, Hanley, & Layer, 2009 ;Leaf et al, 2012 ). Future research should examine child preferences as it impacts play instruction.…”
Section: Prompt Selectionmentioning
confidence: 99%