2006
DOI: 10.2466/pms.102.1.265-274
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

An Examination of Practice and Laterality Effects on the Purdue Pegboard and Moving Beans with Tweezers

Abstract: The purpose of this study was to examine the relation of practice or learning and laterality on the Purdue Pegboard and the Moving Beans with Tweezers test. The subjects were 30 right-handed, healthy young male adults (age: M = 21.1, SD = 1.9 yr.). The subjects performed both tests five times with each hand. A two-way analysis of variance (hand x trial) for scores on the Purdue Pegboard showed that Trial 1 had a significantly lower mean than Trials 4 and 5 with the dominant hand, and scores on Trials 1 and 3 w… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

1
8
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
1
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Our FFT analyses further confirmed the superiority of the right hand with lower tracking error between 0.3 and 1.2 Hz, a frequency range that matches with rather slow (voluntary) visuomotor feedback loops. Overall these results are consistent with previous studies that explored the effect of hand dominance during hand tracking (Simon et al, 1952; Carey et al, 2003; Aoki et al, 2016), as well as other studies investigating reaching movements (Carson et al, 1993; Elliott et al, 1993; Roy et al, 1994; Carey and Liddle, 2013; Schaffer and Sainburg, 2017), and conventional tests of manual dexterity (Bryden and Roy, 2005; Noguchi et al, 2006).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Our FFT analyses further confirmed the superiority of the right hand with lower tracking error between 0.3 and 1.2 Hz, a frequency range that matches with rather slow (voluntary) visuomotor feedback loops. Overall these results are consistent with previous studies that explored the effect of hand dominance during hand tracking (Simon et al, 1952; Carey et al, 2003; Aoki et al, 2016), as well as other studies investigating reaching movements (Carson et al, 1993; Elliott et al, 1993; Roy et al, 1994; Carey and Liddle, 2013; Schaffer and Sainburg, 2017), and conventional tests of manual dexterity (Bryden and Roy, 2005; Noguchi et al, 2006).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Motor control is generally more efficient for the dominant hand than the non-dominant hand. This idea is supported by numerous reports comparing the time to complete tests of manual dexterity (Bryden and Roy, 2005; Noguchi et al, 2006; Wang et al, 2011), as well as reports comparing the accuracy and variability of reaching movements (Carson et al, 1993; Elliott et al, 1993; Roy et al, 1994; Carey and Liddle, 2013; Schaffer and Sainburg, 2017). As for the effect of handedness on predictions, however, this issue has been less explored.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 84%
“…The extent of this variability varies depending on the type of task performed, but also on the limb performing the task. In fact, behavioral research has revealed numerous advantages of the dominant (or preferred) limb in the generation of motor output including an increased strength (Armstrong and Oldham, 1999 ; Farthing et al, 2005 ), rate (Todor and Kyprie, 1980 ; Noguchi et al, 2006 ) and consistency of movement (Peters, 1976 ; Todor et al, 1982 ; Schmidt et al, 2000 ). For instance, Armstrong and Oldham ( 1999 ), when comparing maximum grip forces in healthy subjects, showed that they produced with the dominant arm forces approximately larger by 10% than those produced with the non-dominant arm.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, Armstrong and Oldham ( 1999 ), when comparing maximum grip forces in healthy subjects, showed that they produced with the dominant arm forces approximately larger by 10% than those produced with the non-dominant arm. Also, in reaching tasks, when the rate by which pegs can be moved on a pegboard was evaluated, the dominant hand showed to be superior to the non-dominant one (Noguchi et al, 2006 ). Moreover, numerous finger tapping experiments have demonstrated that the dominant limb (DL) has advantages, in terms of speed and timing consistency, over the non-dominant limb (NDL) when sequential actions are performed at maximal speed (Peters, 1976 ; Todor et al, 1982 ; Schmidt et al, 2000 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Noguchi et al [19] reported that lateral dominance is found in the practice effect in the Moving Beans with Tweezers test from results showing that the practice effect is found only in the nondominant hand. They reasoned that the practice effects was not found in the dominant hand because it is frequently used in movements similar to those in the Moving Beans with Tweezers test in daily life, and therefore did not develop.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%