2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2014.09.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

An onset is an onset: Evidence from abstraction of newly-learned phonotactic constraints

Abstract: Phonotactic constraints are language-specific patterns in the sequencing of speech sounds. Are these constraints represented at the syllable level (ng cannot begin syllables in English) or at the word level (ng cannot begin words)? In a continuous recognition-memory task, participants more often falsely recognized novel test items that followed than violated the training constraints, whether training and test items matched in word structure (one or two syllables) or position of restricted consonants (word-edge… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...

Citation Types

2
22
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
(64 reference statements)
2
22
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Sensitivity to phonotactic constraints is exhibited early (e.g., Jusczyk, Friederici, Wessels, Svenkerud, & Jusczyk, 1993; Jusczyk, Luce, & Charles-Luce, 1994; Zamuner & Kharlamov, 2016 for recent review) and remains flexible, allowing, in some cases, adult second-language learners to be nearly as sensitive to the phonotactics of their second language as to those of their first language, and nearly as sensitive as native speakers of that language (Weber & Cutler, 2006). Moreover, adults quickly learn novel phonotactic constraints such as P-starts and F-ends words from brief auditory (e.g., Bernard, 2015; Chambers, Onishi, & Fisher, 2010; Onishi, Chambers, & Fisher, 2002) or production experience (e.g., Dell, Reed, Adams, & Meyer, 2000; Goldrick, 2004; Goldrick & Larson, 2008; Kittredge & Dell, 2011; Warker & Dell, 2006, 2015; Warker, Dell, Whalen, & Gereg, 2008; Warker, Xu, Dell, & Fisher, 2009), enabling them to respond differentially to novel words like paf and fap that either follow or violate these constraints, as demonstrated by their rates of false recognition, repetition latencies, or production accuracies.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 4 more Smart Citations
“…Sensitivity to phonotactic constraints is exhibited early (e.g., Jusczyk, Friederici, Wessels, Svenkerud, & Jusczyk, 1993; Jusczyk, Luce, & Charles-Luce, 1994; Zamuner & Kharlamov, 2016 for recent review) and remains flexible, allowing, in some cases, adult second-language learners to be nearly as sensitive to the phonotactics of their second language as to those of their first language, and nearly as sensitive as native speakers of that language (Weber & Cutler, 2006). Moreover, adults quickly learn novel phonotactic constraints such as P-starts and F-ends words from brief auditory (e.g., Bernard, 2015; Chambers, Onishi, & Fisher, 2010; Onishi, Chambers, & Fisher, 2002) or production experience (e.g., Dell, Reed, Adams, & Meyer, 2000; Goldrick, 2004; Goldrick & Larson, 2008; Kittredge & Dell, 2011; Warker & Dell, 2006, 2015; Warker, Dell, Whalen, & Gereg, 2008; Warker, Xu, Dell, & Fisher, 2009), enabling them to respond differentially to novel words like paf and fap that either follow or violate these constraints, as demonstrated by their rates of false recognition, repetition latencies, or production accuracies.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, Bernard (2015) found support for syllable-level learning while directly manipulating syllable position by varying word structure and word position between training and test words. For example, participants trained on items like buF.Pak (where the period indicates the syllable boundary and capital letters indicate restricted consonants) distinguished between test items such as Pit.vuF (with F and P in the same syllable positions as in the training items) and Fit.vuP (with F and P in different syllable positions).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations