2001
DOI: 10.1518/001872001775992453
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Anesthesia Alarms in Context: An Observational Study

Abstract: This paper surveys current work on the design of alarms for anesthesia environments and notes some of the problems arising from the need to interpret alarms in context. Anesthetists' responses to audible alarms in the operating room were observed across four types of surgical procedure (laparoscopic, arthroscopic, cardiac, and intracranial) and across three phases of a procedure (induction, maintenance, and emergence). Alarms were classified as (a) requiring a corrective response, (b) being the intended result… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
75
2
1

Year Published

2004
2004
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 94 publications
(80 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
2
75
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Some work environments stand out because of the apparent difficulty of achieving this goal. For example, many researchers have pointed out that the anesthesiologist is poorly supported by the design of the anesthesia machine and the associated patient monitoring systems (Cook & Woods, 1996;Seagull & Sanderson, 2001;Watson, Sanderson, & Russell, 2004). The commonly noted frustration with auditory alarm systems is a symptom of the wrong information (critical boundaries rather than trends) arriving at the wrong time (when many other things may also be going wrong) and in the wrong format (an auditory format that must be silenced for work to proceed effectively).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some work environments stand out because of the apparent difficulty of achieving this goal. For example, many researchers have pointed out that the anesthesiologist is poorly supported by the design of the anesthesia machine and the associated patient monitoring systems (Cook & Woods, 1996;Seagull & Sanderson, 2001;Watson, Sanderson, & Russell, 2004). The commonly noted frustration with auditory alarm systems is a symptom of the wrong information (critical boundaries rather than trends) arriving at the wrong time (when many other things may also be going wrong) and in the wrong format (an auditory format that must be silenced for work to proceed effectively).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Seagull and Sanderson also diff erentiated three diff erent phases in anesthesia procedures (introduction, maintenance and emergence) and found characteristic patterns of alarms and alarm reac tions in each phase [20].…”
Section: Phase Specifi C Settingsmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Only 3 % of all alarms indicated cri tical situations [21]. However, the studies by Kestin [21] and Seagull [20] were l imited by the fact that 5 and 6 diff e rent monitors, re spectively, were used in these observational studies.…”
Section: False Alarm Ratesmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Such automated judgment systems have been found to be valuable, particularly as alerting systems [96], [97].…”
Section: A Taxonomy Of Automated Judgment Supportmentioning
confidence: 99%