2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.070
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Animal signals

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
153
0
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 186 publications
(154 citation statements)
references
References 7 publications
0
153
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This is similar to the findings of Bolhuis, Schouten, Schrama, and Wiegant (2005) that proactive pigs initiated more fights but did not acquire higher social ranks. As outlined above, this could be seen as a dishonest or unreliable signal of fighting ability whereas it is an honest signal of intent (Adams & Mesterton-Gibbons, 1995;Laidre & Johnstone, 2013;Sz amad o, 2000). It may have been that these animals were more willing to engage in aggression (Hofmann & Schildberger, 2001), which would be in line with the measure of aggressiveness from the RI test.…”
Section: Integrating Personality With Contest Theorymentioning
confidence: 96%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This is similar to the findings of Bolhuis, Schouten, Schrama, and Wiegant (2005) that proactive pigs initiated more fights but did not acquire higher social ranks. As outlined above, this could be seen as a dishonest or unreliable signal of fighting ability whereas it is an honest signal of intent (Adams & Mesterton-Gibbons, 1995;Laidre & Johnstone, 2013;Sz amad o, 2000). It may have been that these animals were more willing to engage in aggression (Hofmann & Schildberger, 2001), which would be in line with the measure of aggressiveness from the RI test.…”
Section: Integrating Personality With Contest Theorymentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Guderley & Couture, 2005). Rather than viewing aggressiveness as a component of RHP the aggressiveness displayed by an individual can also be viewed as a signal of intent (Laidre & Johnstone, 2013;Searcy & Nowicki, 2005). More aggressive pigs were more likely to initiate biting, which would reflect an honest signal of their intention to attack.…”
Section: Aggressiveness As a Component Of Rhpmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…These uses of deception, however, bear a common risk: if deceptive signals are used too frequently or out of context, receivers can learn to recognize them and eventually ignore or even punish the signaler [1][2][3]10]. Animals are known to ''negotiate'' such deceptive constraints with genetic adaptations (i.e., polymorphisms) [6] or by opportunistically switching between deceptive and nondeceptive signals [1,7,8].…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Animal communication is often deceptive; however, such dishonesty can become ineffective if it is used too often, is used out of context, or is too easy to detect [1][2][3]. Mimicry is a common form of deception, and most mimics gain the greatest fitness benefits when they are rare compared to their models [3,4].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…2 Behavioural displays have been traditionally defined as conspicuous stereotyped movements performed in special contexts with apparent communicative functions (Shettleworth, 2009). They can provide several kinds of information concurrently (e.g., a display of a chimpanzee male combines both acoustic and visual components) and may have been shaped by natural selection to convey that specific information (Bradbury and Vehrencamp, 1998;Laidre and Johnstone, 2013).…”
Section: A Commentary Onmentioning
confidence: 99%