We examined how the format in which uncertainty information is presented affects two biases in humans' choice behavior. In a computer task, participants were given four common-ratio effect and four commonconsequence effect problems in each of four different formats. In these problems, uncertainty information was described, as percentages (e.g., 80%) or as frequencies (e.g., 16/20), or was experienced, either serially (20 outcomes shown one at a time) or simultaneously (20 outcomes all shown at once). Presenting information as percentages attenuated the common-ratio effect and augmented the common-consequence effect, which suggests that these biases have different underlying mechanisms. Participants' percentage estimates of outcome likelihoods did not differ according to the format in which the information was presented; however, participants' nonverbal estimates of outcome likelihoods differed across formats. The results suggest that uncertainty information presented as percentages is processed differently than the same uncertainty information presented in other formats.The world is predictable, but only probabilistically so. Choices rarely lead to certain outcomes, and an important part of an organism's life involves assessing an option when multiple outcomes are possible. Foraging animals must allocate time across locations, even though there is no guarantee that any one location will provide sustenance at any given time. People must choose which route is likely to be fastest, which course of action is most likely to provide relief from back pain, and which job will provide the best balance of money, security, and enjoyment.In all these choices, there is an element of uncertainty as to which particular outcome will occur, and how this uncertainty is represented and processed determines the choices organisms make.In studies of decision making under uncertainty in humans, people receive information about the value of an outcome and its likelihood of occurrence before being asked to make a decision based on that information. Typically, information is given in a summarized form that includes probabilities and payouts (e.g., ''Lottery A pays $5 with a probability of .75''), and less frequently information is presented as frequencies or in graphical form (E.U. Weber, Shafir, & Blais, 2004). However, as Hertwig, Barron, Weber, and Erev (2004) pointed out, uncertainty information outside the laboratory must often be gathered from personal experience. In addition, direct experience with environmental contingencies is the only way in which nonverbal animals can gather uncertainty information. It is clear that both humans and nonverbal animals are able to extract this information from experience in order to behave adaptively. It follows that a question of primary importance is how decision making varies as a function of how uncertainty information is presented.Humans exhibit decision-making biases in which their choices are inconsistent with normative principles. One might wonder whether such decision biases reflect the f...