2017
DOI: 10.12973/eurasia.2017.01000a
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Animals in Dangerous Postures Enhance Learning, but Decrease Willingness to Protect Animals

Abstract: Animals are the most prevalent subjects for photographs in science textbooks. Many of them are potentially dangerous to humans, and visual exposure to potential threat can influence learning outcomes as well as emotional attachment to these animals. We experimentally investigated the influence of animal posture (aggressive-looking vs. neutral-looking) on 10 -13-year-old pupils' information retention and willingness to support the protection of these animals. We found that information placed below aggressive-lo… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
19
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
(49 reference statements)
2
19
0
Order By: Relevance
“…When an animal species is projected as aggressive, a negative emotional experience can be produced in the public. This negative experience may in turn lead the individual to believe the species is a dangerous agent or threat to human life, bringing about attitudes against its conservation ( Prokop and Fančovičová, 2017 ). On the contrary, if wildlife species are positively seen by children through different media outlets, where the real facts about unpopular animals are shown, it is more likely that fear and disgust decrease, while empathy may grow ( Prokop et al, 2011 ).…”
Section: Final Remarksmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When an animal species is projected as aggressive, a negative emotional experience can be produced in the public. This negative experience may in turn lead the individual to believe the species is a dangerous agent or threat to human life, bringing about attitudes against its conservation ( Prokop and Fančovičová, 2017 ). On the contrary, if wildlife species are positively seen by children through different media outlets, where the real facts about unpopular animals are shown, it is more likely that fear and disgust decrease, while empathy may grow ( Prokop et al, 2011 ).…”
Section: Final Remarksmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recognized means of acquiring knowledge about animals, especially vertebrates, include parental interactions [19, 23, 24]; direct interactions with species in everyday situations [13, 17], including activities such as fishing, feeding animals, and observing wildlife [25]; fictional stories [26]; access to the media [22, 26, 27]; non-formal education strategies such as museums, parks, and zoos; and, especially, access to formal education [3, 20, 25, 28] including images of animals in textbooks [29].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These animals represent common and typical species for each category. We chose only animals displaying neutral postures to avoid biases in responses to aggressive looking animals (Masataka et al, 2010;Prokop & Fančovičová, 2017). Each…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It has been speculated that danger from animals is elicited by certain harmful shapes, such as teeth, claws or spikes (Prokop & Fančovičová, 2017;Souchet & Aubret, 2016;Štefaniková & Prokop, 2015). Indeed, detection of a snake among fear-irrelevant distractors (e.g., flowers, mushrooms) on a touchscreen was consistently faster than vice versa (LoBue & DeLoache, 2008;LoBue & Rakison, 2013;Öhman et al, 2001).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation