1997
DOI: 10.1080/13554799708404034
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Anomia without dyslexia in Chinese

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

2
20
0

Year Published

2000
2000
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 48 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
2
20
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We expected that (1) dissociations between picture naming and oral reading would be observed in Persian‐speaking patients with aphasia; (2) greater impairment when reading words with opaque spellings; and (3) psycholinguistic variables would differentiate picture naming and oral reading. Overall, the group analysis found that oral reading is significantly better preserved than picture naming for most cases consistent with other studies in languages such as Chinese and English (Breen & Warrington, ; Law, Wong, & Chiu, ; Weekes, Chen, et al ., ). This extends this observation to an Indo‐European language with a Semitic script.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We expected that (1) dissociations between picture naming and oral reading would be observed in Persian‐speaking patients with aphasia; (2) greater impairment when reading words with opaque spellings; and (3) psycholinguistic variables would differentiate picture naming and oral reading. Overall, the group analysis found that oral reading is significantly better preserved than picture naming for most cases consistent with other studies in languages such as Chinese and English (Breen & Warrington, ; Law, Wong, & Chiu, ; Weekes, Chen, et al ., ). This extends this observation to an Indo‐European language with a Semitic script.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Law et al . () reported effects of rated AoA and orthography to phonology (OP) consistency as well interaction between these variables on oral reading in a case of acquired dyslexia in Chinese and argued these effects indicated reading via a non‐semantic reading pathway in Chinese (see also Weekes, ; Weekes, Chen, & Gang, ; Weekes, Coltheart, & Gordon, ). Using linear mixed‐effects modelling which considers individual differences across cases as well as fixed effects of item characteristics, Crepaldi, Che, Su, and Luzzatti () found significant independent effects of word frequency and AoA on reading together with significant effects of word frequency and imageability on picture naming.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are four tone marks, "1" for the high tone, "2" for the rising tone, "3" for the falling-rising tone, and "4" for the falling tone. Unlike alphabetic scripts, where phonemes are associated with letters, some scholars hold no phoneme corresponds to any target grapheme (Law & Or, 2001;Siok, Perfetti, Jin, & Tan, 2004;Weekes, Chen, & Gang, 1997;Yin, He, & Weekes, 2005). In other words, the visual-sound-correspondence that resembles the grapheme to phoneme conversion (GPC) used in alphabetic scripts doesn't exist in Putonghua, though some scholars may disagree with this assertion.…”
mentioning
confidence: 83%
“…Although Guo and Zhang mentioned impaired reading in Chinese SD patients, yet a detailed study has not been reported (Guo, Hong, Fu, Yu, & Lu, 2003;Zhang et al, 2008). It has been suggested that the reading in Chinese always relies on the lexical route because there is no GPC rule (Law & Or, 2001;Law, Wong, & Chiu, 2005;Weekes et al, 1997;Yin et al, 2005). Weekes hypothesized a lexical processing model for normal oral reading of Chinese script from print to phonological output.…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%
See 1 more Smart Citation