2004
DOI: 10.1111/j.0092-5853.2004.00592.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Are Choice Experiments Incentive Compatible? A Test with Quality Differentiated Beef Steaks

Abstract: This study compares hypothetical and nonhypothetical responses to choice experiment questions. We test for hypothetical bias in a choice experiment involving beef ribeye steaks with differing quality attributes. In general, hypothetical responses predicted higher probabilities of purchasing beef steaks than nonhypothetical responses. Thus, hypothetical choices overestimate total willingness-to-pay for beef steaks. However, marginal willingness-to-pay for a change in steak quality is, in general, not statistica… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

11
431
1
5

Year Published

2009
2009
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 623 publications
(448 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
11
431
1
5
Order By: Relevance
“…Indeed, the choice architecture surrounding real-world and hypothetical choices has been shown to affect choices, including healthy eating, physical activity, and alcohol use [23][24][25]. Furthermore, if DCEs are not incentive compatible, respondents may try and answer strategically, for example to understate their willingness to pay for public services [26,27]. Behavioral economic research challenges the theory that we are all variants of homo economicus, but there has been limited work exploring if behavioral heuristics influence preferences to a greater or lesser extent in stated preference tasks than in reality [28,29].…”
Section: Background On Validity In Dcesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, the choice architecture surrounding real-world and hypothetical choices has been shown to affect choices, including healthy eating, physical activity, and alcohol use [23][24][25]. Furthermore, if DCEs are not incentive compatible, respondents may try and answer strategically, for example to understate their willingness to pay for public services [26,27]. Behavioral economic research challenges the theory that we are all variants of homo economicus, but there has been limited work exploring if behavioral heuristics influence preferences to a greater or lesser extent in stated preference tasks than in reality [28,29].…”
Section: Background On Validity In Dcesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The empirical evidence of hypothetical bias in a choice experiment is mixed (see, e.g., Carlsson and Martinsson 2001;Lusk and Schroeder 2004;Johansson-Stenman and Svedsäter 2008). To reduce the probability of a hypothetical bias, following Carlsson et al (2005) and List et al (2005), we used a cheap-talk script, as follows:…”
Section: Jena Economic Research Papers 2010 -074mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Rather, it would seem that another behavioural benefit of using real incentives could be a reduced tendency to use opting out as a heuristic. This is however in opposition to results in Lusk and Schroeder (2004), so more research on this issue is warranted. Another decision-making heuristic that could be causing the faster choice task responses obtained from respondents receiving the real incentive would be if respondents just made random choices, i.e.…”
Section: Conclusion and Discussionmentioning
confidence: 72%
“…One central criticism concerns the hypothetical nature of the methods which has been claimed to instigate respondents to overstate their 'true' willingness to pay (WTP) (see e.g. List et al 2006;Lusk and Schroeder 2004;Murphy et al 2005). Behavioural explanations offered for such overstatements relate to the fact that since respondents are not asked to actually pay the stated amounts out of their pockets, they have no incentives to answer truthfully -in accordance with their real preferences that is.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation