2009
DOI: 10.1037/a0016232
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Are we on the same wavelength? Interpersonal complementarity as shared cyclical patterns during interactions.

Abstract: The present study addressed hypotheses about cyclical entrainment between interacting dyad members in their moment-to-moment levels of dominance and affiliation. Using a computer joystick technique, observers recorded the continuous stream of behavior for each partner in 50 mixed-sex dyads, and the data for each dyad were submitted to time-series analyses, including cross-spectral analysis. Although potentially interesting individual differences emerged, in most dyads, partners shared behavior cycles of roughl… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

28
322
1
1

Year Published

2010
2010
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 197 publications
(352 citation statements)
references
References 58 publications
28
322
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, researchers interested in questions of linguistic alignment (e.g., priming; Brennan & Clark, 1996;Cleland & Pickering, 2003;Kousidis & Dorran, 2009;Niederhoffer & Pennebaker, 2002;Reitter, Moore, & Keller, 2010) will find the two-channel recording method amenable to their research (e.g., transcription; Kreuz & Riordan, 2011). Additionally, by combining the FDM with pre-or postinteraction questionnaires, researchers interested in affective synchrony (e.g., Chartrand & Bargh, 1999;Lakin & Chartrand, 2003;Miles et al, 2011;Sadler et al, 2009;Valdesolo & Desteno, 2011) may begin to investigate questions of affective alignment in conjunction with other channels of communication. By combining research into these and other channels, the field can better understand the functions of interpersonal synchrony.…”
Section: Future Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, researchers interested in questions of linguistic alignment (e.g., priming; Brennan & Clark, 1996;Cleland & Pickering, 2003;Kousidis & Dorran, 2009;Niederhoffer & Pennebaker, 2002;Reitter, Moore, & Keller, 2010) will find the two-channel recording method amenable to their research (e.g., transcription; Kreuz & Riordan, 2011). Additionally, by combining the FDM with pre-or postinteraction questionnaires, researchers interested in affective synchrony (e.g., Chartrand & Bargh, 1999;Lakin & Chartrand, 2003;Miles et al, 2011;Sadler et al, 2009;Valdesolo & Desteno, 2011) may begin to investigate questions of affective alignment in conjunction with other channels of communication. By combining research into these and other channels, the field can better understand the functions of interpersonal synchrony.…”
Section: Future Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The same idea, however, can also be applied to the description of the moment-to-moment changes in the participants' behaviors occurring during dyadic interaction. To this aim, Pamela Sadler and her co-workers [49,74] have recently developed a method that relies on naïve observers' ratings of interpersonal behavior, based on the assumption that the circumplex structure of interpersonal behaviors is an arrangement intuitively accessible to raters [99]. The method uses a computer interface with which raters assess the participants' dominant and affiliative interpersonal behaviors (see Method for further details), and it has been shown to be highly reliable and to have strong convergent and discriminant validity with more traditional interpersonal measures [74], such as the Social Behavior Inventory [57].…”
Section: Interpersonal Dynamics During Dyadic Interactionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…By using the joystick method developed by [74] we compared the patterns characteristic for neurotypical (NT) male dyads (NT dyads) with the ones characteristic for male dyads (AS dyads) where one participant is a neurotypical and the other has the autistic spectrum disorder that is generally known as Asperger's syndrome (AS). Given that AS is characterized by deficits in social interaction [28,85], we hypothesized that there would be identifiable differences in the extent to and the specific ways in which AS participants and their NT co-participants would match each other's dominant and affiliative interpersonal behaviors.…”
Section: Doi: 107243/2054-992x-4-2mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Interpersonal attitudes and personality characteristics have also been widely studied in relation to nonverbal behaviour (Argyle and Dean 1965;Burgoon and Poire 1999;Sadler et al 2009;Scherer 1978;Tusing and Dillard 2000). In this context, several authors have argued that nonverbal behaviour expresses two major dimensions that regulate interpersonal relationships: affiliation and dominance (Kiesler 1983;Mehrabian and Ksionzky 1972;Wiggins et al 1988).…”
Section: The Production and Perception Of Social Signals And Cuesmentioning
confidence: 99%