2011
DOI: 10.2466/11.17.21.28.pms.113.4.311-330
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Articulatory Behaviors of Nonnative Speakers: Role of L2 Proficiency and Accent Modification

Abstract: To sound native-like, nonnative speakers need to approximate the articulatory patterns of native speakers. When nonnative speakers deviate from native speakers' production patterns, it gives rise to a nonnative accent. This study examines how proficiency in a second language (L2) is related to processing skills in L2, as measured by the accuracy of consonant production. 10 monolingual speakers of American English, 10 Bengali-English bilinguals with high proficiency in English, and 10 Bengali-English bilinguals… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Overall L2 proficiency has been shown to predict how nativelike L2 speech production is. More proficient L2 learners speak faster and make fewer errors (Kormos & Dénes 2004); they have better speech motor control (Nip & Blumenfeld 2015); they show greater segmental accuracy (Chakraborty, Domsch & Gonzales 2011) and adopt L2 articulatory patterns (e.g., Flege, Schirru & MacKay, 2003). In addition, based on our long-term experience with the target learner population, we believe motivation to be an essential factor in compensating for the input handicap.…”
Section: Measuring Attainmentthe Monolingual Reference Pointmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Overall L2 proficiency has been shown to predict how nativelike L2 speech production is. More proficient L2 learners speak faster and make fewer errors (Kormos & Dénes 2004); they have better speech motor control (Nip & Blumenfeld 2015); they show greater segmental accuracy (Chakraborty, Domsch & Gonzales 2011) and adopt L2 articulatory patterns (e.g., Flege, Schirru & MacKay, 2003). In addition, based on our long-term experience with the target learner population, we believe motivation to be an essential factor in compensating for the input handicap.…”
Section: Measuring Attainmentthe Monolingual Reference Pointmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Cebrian (2007) and Zhang (2019) used a task in which bilingual listeners heard an /hVd/ non-word and were asked to repeat the word in an /hVb/ frame, thus forcing the listener to abstract away from the phonetic signal in the stimulus. Chakraborty et al (2011) used 16 different non-words of different lengths, containing a range of phonemes, in a non-word repetition task to test the articulation of English-Bengali bilinguals. Thus, although we have not seen non-word repetition used with naïve listeners, and although there may be no consensus on the purpose of using non-word repetition in assessing child language development, it is nonetheless a task that approximates the earliest stages of L2 classroom exposure and is suitable for use with naïve listeners and L2 learners alike.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%