Some children show emotion that is not consistent with normative appraisal of the context and can therefore be defined as context inappropriate (CI). The authors used individual growth curve modeling and hierarchical multiple regression analyses to examine whether CI anger predicts differences in hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity, as manifest in salivary cortisol measures. About 23% of the 360 children (ages 6-10 years, primarily 7-8) showed at least 1 expression of CI anger in situations designed to elicit positive affect. Expression of anger across 2 positive assessments was less common (around 4%). CI anger predicted the hypothesized lower levels of cortisol beyond that attributed to context appropriate anger. Boys' CI anger predicted lower morning cortisol and flatter slopes. Results suggest that this novel approach to studying children's emotion across varying contexts can provide insight into affective style.
Keywordscortisol; emotion; children; context; anger Emotional processes are integral to the development of behavior problems (Cicchetti, Ackerman, & Izard, 1995;Cole, Michel, & O'Donnell-Teti, 1994;Keenan, 2000). However, the fund of knowledge about behavioral and physiological correlates of maladaptive emotional responses is predominately based on observations of emotional reactivity to situations that normatively elicit a particular discrete emotion (context appropriate [CA] affect; e.g., Kagan, Reznick, Snidman, Gibbons, & Johnson, 1988). Early differences in CA emotional responses modestly predict levels of maladaptive behavior (Prior, Smart, Sanson, & Oberklaid, 2000;Rende, 1993) and do not always predict behavior with much specificity (Biederman et al., 1990(Biederman et al., , 1993. Therefore, we might overlook potential differences in affective style by limiting assessment of emotional responses to appropriate contexts such as fear to unfamiliar persons and novel objects (Davidson, Jackson, & Kalin, 2000).Although emotional expressions can be associated with prototypical incentives (i.e., fear with perceived threat, anger with blocked goals, sadness with lost goals, pleasure with safety; see Lazarus, 1991), emotional expression is not a simple reaction to the immediate context. In a given context, it is unlikely that one will observe the expression of a particular emotion by all individuals or by the same individual consistently (Ekman, 1984;Frijda, 1986;Scherer, © 1988). Emotion is also influenced by personal concerns or dispositions to express an emotion (i.e., temperament; Goldsmith & Campos, 1982;Lazarus, 1991), genetics (Deater-Deckard, Petrill, & Thompson, 2007;Goldsmith & Lemery, 2000), or environmental factors (e.g., cultural differences in socialization; Camras, Bakeman, Chen, Norris, & Cain, 2006). Therefore, some children will show emotional responses that are not concordant with normative appraisal of the context (context inappropriate [CI] affect). Affective responses are the observed behavioral expressions of emotion in a given situation. Although what is e...