2004
DOI: 10.1111/j.0079-1636.2004.00131.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Assumptions Behind Grammatical Approaches To Code‐Switching: When The Blueprint Is A Red Herring

Abstract: Many of the so-called 'grammars' of code-switching are based on various underlying assumptions, e.g. that informal speech can be adequately or appropriately described in terms of ''grammar''; that deep, rather than surface, structures are involved in code-switching; that one 'language' is the 'base' or 'matrix'; and that constraints derived from existing data are universal and predictive. We question these assumptions on several grounds. First, 'grammar' is arguably distinct from the processes driving speech p… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
24
0
2

Year Published

2009
2009
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 59 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
1
24
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…They also confirm the feasibility-and more important, the necessity!-of distinguishing them, not from other borrowings, as they are identical to them in linguistic structure, but from code-switches, single-and multiword. This necessity does not emerge from any theory-internal considerations, as alleged by, for example, Bentahila and Davies (1998), Clyne (2003), Eliasson (1989), Gardner-Chloros andEdwards (2004), Jacobson, (1998), Muysken (2000), Nishimura (1995), and Santorini and Mahootian (1995), but from the simple fact that these are different strategies, with different underlying structures, and generated by different grammars.…”
Section: Discussion a N D C O N C L U S I O N Smentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They also confirm the feasibility-and more important, the necessity!-of distinguishing them, not from other borrowings, as they are identical to them in linguistic structure, but from code-switches, single-and multiword. This necessity does not emerge from any theory-internal considerations, as alleged by, for example, Bentahila and Davies (1998), Clyne (2003), Eliasson (1989), Gardner-Chloros andEdwards (2004), Jacobson, (1998), Muysken (2000), Nishimura (1995), and Santorini and Mahootian (1995), but from the simple fact that these are different strategies, with different underlying structures, and generated by different grammars.…”
Section: Discussion a N D C O N C L U S I O N Smentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the formal study of intra-sentential CS, linguistic creativity has not played a prominent role given that for most scholars, CS primarily entails the combination of two autonomous grammatical systems that remain discrete in bilingual speech (for relevant discussion, see Gardner-Chloros and Edwards [3]). This suggests that CS is restricted to pre-existing structures and/or patterns in the component languages, and any degree of hybridity is envisioned as indexical of convergence.…”
Section: In Introducing This Inaugural Specialmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We were initially drawn to the study of these mixed verb constructions as they allow us to examine not only how bi/multilinguals seamlessly integrate their languages but how they creatively abstract away from pre-existing structures and/or patterns to devise novel ones. We know that a distinctive characteristic of mixed verbs is that they often evince structural features that cannot be easily attributed to one language or another; hence, revealing the flexibility, dynamism, and creativity of CS [3,[8][9][10].…”
Section: In Introducing This Inaugural Specialmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As of this writing, however, not only is there no general consensus on what linguistic conditions constrain CS, but some scholars insist that the very search for them is futile (e.g., Bokamba, 1989;Gardner-Chloros and Edwards, 2004); others maintain that there are none (Chan, 2009;MacSwan, 1999;Mahootian, 1993), and still others, that constraints on CS may vary from one community to another (Bhatt, 1997;Muysken, 2000;Schindler et al, 2008).…”
Section: A 'Grammar' Of Csmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, blind adherence to Occam's razor (e.g., Chan, 2009;MacSwan, 1999) and the quest for a 'unified account' (Gardner-Chloros, 2009;Myers Scotton, 2006;Winford, 2009) enjoin us to ignore fundamental differences among different manifestations of language contact, and to treat them allat our peril!as one and the same. Section A 'Grammar' of CS detailed the tradition of appealing to the syntactic theories currently in vogue as models for CS behavior.…”
Section: Why Consensus Remains Elusivementioning
confidence: 99%