2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2011.01.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Attention and the identification of parafoveal words in school-age children and adults

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

9
10
0
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 78 publications
(92 reference statements)
9
10
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Visual search for words became more efficient in both the literal and categorical search tasks both because older adolescents gazed less often at non-target words during the search and because they could reject non-target words more expeditiously once they were fixated. This supports the assumption that the progressive automation of word decoding facilitates the rejection of non-target words and makes more mental resources available for parafoveal/peripheral word processing (Siéroff and Riva, 2011) and top-down guidance within word lists. In addition, the lexical quality hypothesis (Perfetti, 1992, 2007; Verhoeven and Perfetti, 2011; Verhoeven et al, 2011) states that the acquisition of word decoding is accompanied by an experience-related increase in the lexical quality of word representations within semantic memory, which would go on during the whole adolescence and through to adulthood.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 77%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Visual search for words became more efficient in both the literal and categorical search tasks both because older adolescents gazed less often at non-target words during the search and because they could reject non-target words more expeditiously once they were fixated. This supports the assumption that the progressive automation of word decoding facilitates the rejection of non-target words and makes more mental resources available for parafoveal/peripheral word processing (Siéroff and Riva, 2011) and top-down guidance within word lists. In addition, the lexical quality hypothesis (Perfetti, 1992, 2007; Verhoeven and Perfetti, 2011; Verhoeven et al, 2011) states that the acquisition of word decoding is accompanied by an experience-related increase in the lexical quality of word representations within semantic memory, which would go on during the whole adolescence and through to adulthood.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 77%
“…The shortening of gaze durations observed between fifth and seventh grade may reflect the acquisition of fluent word decoding abilities by sixth grade (Vellutino et al, 1981; Verhoeven and van Leeuwe, 2008; Verhoeven et al, 2011). The reduction of the number of gazes may correspond to an increased ability to process word features in the parafoveal/peripheral visual field (Perfetti, 2007; Siéroff and Riva, 2011; Verhoeven and Perfetti, 2011), as already suggested for the literal task (see discussion of Experiment 1).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 55%
“…Sié roff and Riva (2011) provide an argument in favor of the attentional bias theory, as they show the same larger distractor effect on LVF words than on RVF words in young children and adults, although the effect of the distractor is globally much stronger in children indicating less automatic reading. If the asymmetric distractor effect was explained by automatic word processing of the left hemisphere, then one would expect a greater asymmetric distractor effect in adults for whom reading is more automatic.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…A first attentional effect refers to the fact that when a word and a distractor are simultaneously presented in opposite visual fields, word identification is more impaired by presentation of the distractor to the RVF (i.e., the visual field in which the attention is mainly allocated) than to the LVF (Sié roff & Urbanski, 2002;Sié roff & Riva, 2011). Interestingly, such distractor effect is reversed when both the word and the distractor were presented in foveal vision, attached to each other in such a way that the word was presented to the left or to the right of the fixation position (Van der Haegen & Brysbaert, 2011).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Studies using the spatial-cueing paradigm and visual halffield presentation have demonstrated a larger cueing effect (difference between a condition in which the cue is in the same location as the word and a condition in which the cue is in the opposite location) for LVF than for RVF words (Ducrot & Grainger, 2007;Gatheron & Siéroff, 1999;Mondor & Bryden, 1992;Nicholls & Wood, 1998), and often no cueing effect at all for the RVF words (e.g., Mondor & Bryden, 1992;Nicholls & Wood, 1998). Both the attentional-bias and attentional-advantage hypotheses can explain the cueing effect asymmetry (Siéroff & Riva, 2011), but only the latter can explain the lack of a cueing effect in the RVF.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%