2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.07.013
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Attention capture is temporally stable: Evidence from mixed-model correlations

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

4
10
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

2
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
4
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We used color singletons (one red stimulus among green stimuli, or one green stimulus among red stimuli) to trigger stimulus-driven attention shifts (cf. Theeuwes, 1992 ; Weichselbaum et al, 2018 ). There were three different configurations: the singleton was the target (attention at the target position; target singleton), the singleton was one of the distractors (attention at a distractor position; distractor singleton), and no singleton present (half of the stimuli in one color, other half in the other color, with color changes between all adjacent positions).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…We used color singletons (one red stimulus among green stimuli, or one green stimulus among red stimuli) to trigger stimulus-driven attention shifts (cf. Theeuwes, 1992 ; Weichselbaum et al, 2018 ). There were three different configurations: the singleton was the target (attention at the target position; target singleton), the singleton was one of the distractors (attention at a distractor position; distractor singleton), and no singleton present (half of the stimuli in one color, other half in the other color, with color changes between all adjacent positions).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“… Chica et al, 2012 ). Stimuli can draw our attention in two different ways ( Egeth and Yantis, 1997 ; Wolfe et al, 2003 ; Connor et al, 2004 ; Weichselbaum et al, 2018 ): in an intentional, goal-directed way (top–down/endogenous) or in an automatic, stimulus-driven way (bottom–up/exogenous). In top–down capture, only a stimulus matching the goals and/or search intentions of the observer will capture attention; for example, when looking for a tomato in the supermarket, only red and round objects will be selected for further processing (e.g., Duncan and Humphreys, 1989 ; Folk et al, 1992 ; Büsel et al, 2018 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These studies collectively identified that response time exhibits poor reliability over experience-driven attention effects. However, in Anderson and Kim (2019), value-driven oculomotor capture exhibited strong test-retest reliability, suggesting that oculomotor capture may be more sensitive and reliable in contrast to oculomotor fixation times and even more so when compared with manual response times (Anderson & Kim, 2019; Weichselbaum et al, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…In addition, in a control block, color singletons—a unique blue singleton distractor among the red target and green nonsingleton distractors—captured attention and delayed search times among Korean and German speakers, demonstrating that stimulus-driven capture of attention by a salient local color difference was the same in both groups (cf. Itti & Koch, 2001; Itti et al, 1998; Theeuwes, 1992; Weichselbaum et al, 2018). Together, these findings are in line with a well-practiced habitual generalization of stimulus-driven attention capture by linguistically relevant distinctions to a nonlinguistic visual search task (cf.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%