2008
DOI: 10.1037/a0012476
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Attention, gaze shifting, and dual-task interference from phonological encoding in spoken word planning.

Abstract: Controversy exists about whether dual-task interference from word planning reflects structural bottleneck or attentional control factors. Here, participants named pictures whose names could or could not be phonologically prepared, and they manually responded to arrows presented away from (Experiment 1), or superimposed onto, the pictures (Experiments 2 and 3); or they responded to tones (Experiment 4). Pictures and arrows/tones were presented at stimulus onset asynchronies of 0, 300, and 1,000 ms. Earlier rese… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

9
111
2

Year Published

2010
2010
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

4
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 50 publications
(122 citation statements)
references
References 66 publications
(340 reference statements)
9
111
2
Order By: Relevance
“…els of spoken word production (e.g., Abdel Rahman & Melinger, 2009, Damian & Martin, 1999, Levelt et al, 1999, Roelofs, 1992, 2003, 2007, 2008a, 2008b, Schriefers et al, 1990, Starreveld & La Heij, 1996.…”
Section: The Locus Of the Semantic Interference Effectmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…els of spoken word production (e.g., Abdel Rahman & Melinger, 2009, Damian & Martin, 1999, Levelt et al, 1999, Roelofs, 1992, 2003, 2007, 2008a, 2008b, Schriefers et al, 1990, Starreveld & La Heij, 1996.…”
Section: The Locus Of the Semantic Interference Effectmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A prominent account of this effect places it at the stage of lexical selection (e.g., Levelt et al, 1999). This account has been computationally implemented in several models, including the WEAVERϩϩ model (Levelt et al, 1999;Roelofs, 1992Roelofs, , 2003Roelofs, , 2007Roelofs, , 2008aRoelofs, , 2008b and the model of Starreveld and La Heij (1996).The assumption that the semantic interference effect arises during lexical selection was recently challenged by Dell'Acqua, Job, Peressotti, and Pascali (2007). These authors used PWI as part of a psychological refractory period (PRP) procedure (Pashler, 1984(Pashler, , 1994 to determine at which stage the semantic interference effect emerged.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It is, for instance, possible that there are specific components in the naming process that rely particularly strongly on updating ability. For instance, it has often been proposed that conceptual planning processes and self-monitoring processes require processing capacity (e.g., Levelt, 1989;Oomen & Postma, 2002), whereas lexical access, though not an automatic process (e.g., Cook & Meyer, 2008;Ferreira & Pashler, 2002;Roelofs, 2008a), might be lower in capacity demands. Updating ability might specifically affect the efficiency of the conceptual processes, but not so much the lexical retrieval processes.…”
Section: Contribution Of Updating Abilitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These are general cognitive processes that define and maintain the individual's goals, recruit appropriate perceptual and response mechanisms, and monitor their performance (e.g., Norman & Shallice, 1986;Posner & Petersen, 1990). When we speak, we need to choose our words wisely (e.g., considering our goals and the common ground between interlocutors; Nilsen & Graham, 2009;Ye & Zhou, 2009), allocate sufficient processing capacity to our speech planning processes (e.g., Cook & Meyer, 2008;Ferreira & Pashler, 2002;Roelofs, 2008aRoelofs, , 2008b, and monitor our speech output for appropriateness and correctness. We also need to choose and maintain an appropriate speech rate and register (e.g., child-directed speech or the formal style required for a sermon, see Meyer, Konopka, Wheeldon, & van der Meulen, 2012).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%