IntroductionThe ability of attention to modulate pain processing [32,51,57,66] makes it a key factor in how people cope with pain. Distraction, defined as directing attention away from pain, is probably one of the most intuitively and commonly used coping strategies [33]. Although distraction seems to be omnipresent in pain treatment programs for acute and chronic pain [20,41], results on its efficacy are variable. Some studies found that distraction reduces pain [36,39,44,57,58,65], whereas other studies reported no effect [25,35] or even counter-productive effects [8,21]. An understanding of the conditions under which distraction works is therefore required. Both pain-related factors (e.g., intensity, novelty, threat) and factors related to the task towards which attention is directed (e.g., task difficulty, motivational significance) are likely to play a role [19,32,66].A tendency to selectively attend to pain has been argued to hamper the efficacy of distraction [15,32,72], and is largely discussed in the context of hypervigilance and attentional bias towards pain-related information [15,31]. Indeed, it is reasonable to assume that an attentional bias towards pain-related information may bring along difficulties to direct attention away from pain and, hence, result in a failure of distraction to diminish the experience of pain. However, there is no direct test of this idea. Most often, studies on attentional bias in pain aimed to validate the phenomenon (e.g. [23,34,45,52]), or attempted to identify its antecedent conditions (e.g. catastrophic thinking about pain, state/trait anxiety) [4,46,47,62]. In this study we focussed upon one of the putative consequences of We hypothesized that participants would show an attentional bias towards both cues signalling the experimental pain stimulus and words describing this pain stimulus. Furthermore, we expected that self-reported pain would be less when attention was directed away from pain than when it was directed towards pain. The crucial hypothesis of this study, however, was that a larger attentional bias would be associated with smaller effects of distraction. Finally, we wanted to explore whether an attentional bias towards pain-related information would mediate the relationship between catastrophic thinking about pain and the efficacy of distraction.4
Method
ParticipantsThe sample consisted of 56 undergraduate students from Ghent University, who received course credits for participation (45 females; Mage = 18.1years, SD = 1.1; 55 Caucasian) with normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Exclusion criteria included a self-reported current pain problem (e.g. fibromyalgia, back pain) or a self-reported current psychiatric problem (e.g., psychosis, anxiety disorder).Furthermore, all participants were required to have Dutch as a first language, because the experiment, including all instructions and stimulus materials, was undertaken in Dutch. Based on these criteria, two persons were excluded due to a headache on time of testing. One additional participant was excluded...