2009
DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.35.1.1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Attentional capture by salient color singleton distractors is modulated by top-down dimensional set.

Abstract: Three experiments examined whether salient color singleton distractors automatically interfere with the detection singleton form targets in visual search (e.g., J. Theeuwes, 1992), or whether the degree of interference is top-down modulable. In Experiments 1 and 2, observers started with a pure block of trials, which contained either never a distractor or always a distractor (0% or 100% distractors)--varying the opportunity to learn distractor suppression. In the subsequent trial blocks, the proportion of dist… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

28
196
3

Year Published

2012
2012
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 169 publications
(227 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
28
196
3
Order By: Relevance
“…In Tsal and Makovski's (2006) study, there were no significant changes in overall response times or accuracy when attentional resources were devoted to expected distractor locations. Therefore, the present data show that access to explicit information about nontarget properties is surprisingly detrimental, because "search and destroy" can be a slow, counterproductive process (though foreknowledge of salient distractor probability may be an exception; see, e.g., Moher, Abrams, Egeth, Yantis, & Stuphorn, 2011;Müller, Geyer, Zehetleitner, & Krummenacher, 2009). …”
Section: Inhibition Of Features and Locationsmentioning
confidence: 69%
“…In Tsal and Makovski's (2006) study, there were no significant changes in overall response times or accuracy when attentional resources were devoted to expected distractor locations. Therefore, the present data show that access to explicit information about nontarget properties is surprisingly detrimental, because "search and destroy" can be a slow, counterproductive process (though foreknowledge of salient distractor probability may be an exception; see, e.g., Moher, Abrams, Egeth, Yantis, & Stuphorn, 2011;Müller, Geyer, Zehetleitner, & Krummenacher, 2009). …”
Section: Inhibition Of Features and Locationsmentioning
confidence: 69%
“…Recent findings suggest that this is a plausible explanation (Folk & Remington, 2007;Geyer, Müller, & Krummenacher, 2008;Müller, Geyer, Zehetleitner, & Krummenacher, 2009;Neo & Chua, 2006). For example, Folk and Remington (2007) had participants search for a colour target and presented a colour or onset cue prior to each target.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Even more striking distractor frequency effects can be seen in a visual search paradigm in which a salient distractor singleton is presented on a proportion of trials. Geyer and colleagues (Geyer, Müller & Krummenacher, 2008) report that salient singletons cause distraction when they appear on 20% or 50% of trials, but distraction is completely eliminated when they appear on 80% of trials, despite the singletons being more salient than the target (Müller et al, 2009). Finally, in a set of spatial cueing experiments, distractors in unattended locations slowed responses when they appeared rarely (on less than 20% of trials) but not when they appeared frequently (on 75% of trials; Neo & Chua, 2006).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Emotional distractions plague us all, and in disorders such as depression, anxiety, and addiction, they can be overwhelming (Cisler & Koster, 2010;De Raedt & Koster, 2010;Field & Cox, 2008). Non-emotional distractors are known to disrupt performance (Forster & Lavie, 2008a,2008b, but they can also be controlled if we know to expect them (Braver, 2012;Müller, Geyer, Zehetleitner & Krummenacher, 2009) Can we ever control emotional distractions as effectively as those that are more mundane? …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%