2017
DOI: 10.1111/mila.12131
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Attentive Visual Reference

Abstract: Many have held that when a person visually attends to an object, her visual system deploys a representation that designates the object. Call the referential link between such representations and the objects they designate attentive visual reference. In this article I offer an account of attentive visual reference. I argue that the object representations deployed in visual attention-which I call attentive visual object representations (AVORs)-refer directly, and are akin to indexicals. Then I turn to the issue … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

2
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 79 publications
(73 reference statements)
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Much more would need to be said here, of course. Alternatively, Green (2017) proposes weakening the application of discrimination conditions in the face of such cases: "to visually refer to an object that does not occupy one's entire field of vision, it is necessary to circumscribe that object" (29-30, n. 42).…”
Section: Chameleonmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Much more would need to be said here, of course. Alternatively, Green (2017) proposes weakening the application of discrimination conditions in the face of such cases: "to visually refer to an object that does not occupy one's entire field of vision, it is necessary to circumscribe that object" (29-30, n. 42).…”
Section: Chameleonmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We can distinguish the features used to pick out an object in the first place from the features used to determine how the object persists over time. Green (2017) argues, for instance, that attribution of structural shape properties sets constraints on reference locking, since it is needed to distinguish perceptual reference to parts from perceptual reference to whole bodies. However, this view does not entail that shape information is always used in reference maintenance.…”
Section: Transsaccadic Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The locking of reference to an object occurs “through a brute causal mechanism” (Pylyshyn 2009, 269), such that the properties to which the mechanism is sensitive need not “be encoded and entered into an object file in order for an index to be captured or an object file to be created” (Pylyshyn 2007, 81; emphasis his; cf. Green 2017).…”
Section: Three Models Of the Perceptual Reference‐attribution Relationmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…I argued in the previous section that our experiences do differentiate occluded parts of the scene from their background. Following Strawson (1959) and Evans (1982), another commonly proposed requirement is that one be able to “re‐identify” the item of which one is aware or else track it as it moves and changes (Dickie, 2011; Green, 2017). Much has already been made of the attentional capacity to track whole objects as they move behind occluders; but we also have visual capacities to track partial fragments of objects as those fragments move, change, and become visible or hidden.…”
Section: De Re Awareness Of Hidden Partsmentioning
confidence: 99%