2021
DOI: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101695
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Attitudes and emotions as predictors of support for wolf management

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We encourage adaptation of the present sampling strategy to capture a wider range of behaviors for specific groups of interest and, where possible, to assess those behaviors across multiple points in time (i.e., employ a longitudinal approach). We also acknowledge that other mechanisms may shape beliefs and behaviors beyond those examined herein––including one's level of support for specific management actions (e.g., lethal control) (Manfredo et al., 2021) and the affective components of such judgments (Vaske et al., 2021). Similarly, despite controlling for a wide range of cognitive, demographic, and place‐based characteristics––many of which have been quantified throughout the literature in equally diverse ways––the effect of some factors identified in other models (e.g., wildlife tolerance model [Kansky et al., 2016]) remains untested.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…We encourage adaptation of the present sampling strategy to capture a wider range of behaviors for specific groups of interest and, where possible, to assess those behaviors across multiple points in time (i.e., employ a longitudinal approach). We also acknowledge that other mechanisms may shape beliefs and behaviors beyond those examined herein––including one's level of support for specific management actions (e.g., lethal control) (Manfredo et al., 2021) and the affective components of such judgments (Vaske et al., 2021). Similarly, despite controlling for a wide range of cognitive, demographic, and place‐based characteristics––many of which have been quantified throughout the literature in equally diverse ways––the effect of some factors identified in other models (e.g., wildlife tolerance model [Kansky et al., 2016]) remains untested.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Negative emotions, overall, were not the predominant emotional states expressed. Even though we could reasonably expect rural populations to express higher levels of anger as was recently described in Illinois, United States (Vaske et al., 2021), this was, overall, not the case. This finding suggests that the emotion of anger, which is a reaction associated with perceived injustice (Nelson et al., 2016) and often mentioned in the heated debates of wolf conservation, is not strongly associated with the wolf itself, but may rather be the result of human conflicts over wolf management (König et al., 2020).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 65%
“…Negative emotions, overall, were not the predominant emotional states expressed. Even though we could reasonably expect rural populations to express higher levels of anger as was recently described in Illinois, United States (Vaske et al, 2021), this was, overall, not the case. This finding suggests that the emotion of anger, which is a reaction associated with perceived injustice (Nelson et al, 2016) and often mentioned in the TA B L E 2 Results of ordinal regressions using cumulative link mixed models with emotional intensity as the dependent variable (ordinal variable), with context, wolf presence and their interaction as fixed effects and individual ID as a random factor.…”
Section: The Diversity Of Emotional Statesmentioning
confidence: 69%
“…In this study, the decisions underlying the subscale divisions were based on the relevant international literature [ [23] , [24] , [25] ]. The emotion management subscale was divided into five dimensions: emotional cognition (B1) [ [26] , [27] , [28] ], emotion perception (B2), emotion influence (B3) [ [29] , [30] , [31] , [32] ], emotion control (B4) [ [33] , [34] , [35] , [36] , [37] ], and emotion regulation (B5) [ [38] , [39] , [40] , [41] ]; the benefit evaluation subscale was divided into outcome evaluation (A1) and process evaluation (A2). The question items initially 56 were developed mainly from our review and analysis of the previously mentioned master's and doctoral dissertations [ [25] , [26] , [27] ] relevant academic journals [ 22 , 23 , 26 ], the emotion measure ment scale [ 24 , 28 ]in the Sports Science Common Psychological Scale Evaluation Manual (edited by Mao Zhixiong), and published psychology and sports psychology books.…”
Section: Scale Development Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 99%