2003
DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.01454
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Attribution of Dispositional States by 12-Month-Olds

Abstract: The ability to interpret the behavior of other individuals is essential for effective social functioning. Many investigators now believe that even young infants can recognize that agents act toward goals. Here we report three experiments suggesting that 12-month-old infants not only can recognize goal-related action, but also can interpret future actions of an actor on the basis of previously witnessed behavior in another context. The possibility that this inference is made through the attribution of mental st… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

18
380
2
2

Year Published

2005
2005
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 458 publications
(402 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
18
380
2
2
Order By: Relevance
“…First, they suggest that, by 13.5 months of age, infants can attribute to agents not only dispositions involving particular agents and objects (e.g., Kuhlmeier et al, 2003;Luo & Baillargeon, 2005, in press;Onishi & Baillargeon, 2002;Premack & Premack, 1997;Song et al, 2005), but also dispositions involving particular actions. The infants in the present research attributed to the actor an inclination to slide or lift objects, and they used this disposition to predict, when new objects were introduced in test, which object the actor would select (and hence which goal-directed actions the actor would perform to obtain that object).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…First, they suggest that, by 13.5 months of age, infants can attribute to agents not only dispositions involving particular agents and objects (e.g., Kuhlmeier et al, 2003;Luo & Baillargeon, 2005, in press;Onishi & Baillargeon, 2002;Premack & Premack, 1997;Song et al, 2005), but also dispositions involving particular actions. The infants in the present research attributed to the actor an inclination to slide or lift objects, and they used this disposition to predict, when new objects were introduced in test, which object the actor would select (and hence which goal-directed actions the actor would perform to obtain that object).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These findings, together with those cited above, raise many interesting questions for future research. For example, are these dispositions best understood as primitive mental states or as behavioral tendencies (for discussion, see Kuhlmeier et al, 2003)? What kinds of positive and negative dispositions are infants of different ages able to attribute to agents?…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…There is also evidence that 3-4-year-old toddlers respond like adults to the perception of Heider and Simmel type of stimuli [35]. Recently, when showed a triangle and a square whose motions were automatically seen respectively as 'helping' and as 'hindering' a circle move up a slope, 12-month-old infants exhibited a clear preference for the former over the latter [36].…”
Section: Motor Simulation Motor Intentions and Prior Intentionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…By contrast, geometrical stimuli form a homogeneous class of entities. Seeing geometrical stimuli move in relation to one another causes in humans a 'perceptual social illusion', that is, an illusion of social interactions guided by social intentions [33][34][35][36]. But given that the motion of geometrical stimuli is non-biological, it follows that the process whereby social intentions are represented and ascribed cannot be by matching the observed motions onto one's own motor repertoire, that is, by simulation in the narrow sense.…”
Section: Motor Simulation Motor Intentions and Prior Intentionsmentioning
confidence: 99%