2007
DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2006.02.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Auditory Stroop reveals implicit gender associations in adults and children

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4

Citation Types

8
49
0
1

Year Published

2007
2007
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 58 publications
(58 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
8
49
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…A sentence was classified as an associative strategy if the sentence did not interactively incorporate both nouns (e.g., "Fish and seaweed are in the ocean"). In line with the previous research (Banaji and Hardin 1996;Most et al 2007), Kee and Guttentag (1994) found that children (1) were significantly faster at creating a sentence, (2) used more elaborative strategies, and (3) remembered significantly more pairs then when they were presented with congruent noun pairs (e.g., "bush-garden") than incongruent noun pairs (e.g., "glass-elbow").…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 83%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…A sentence was classified as an associative strategy if the sentence did not interactively incorporate both nouns (e.g., "Fish and seaweed are in the ocean"). In line with the previous research (Banaji and Hardin 1996;Most et al 2007), Kee and Guttentag (1994) found that children (1) were significantly faster at creating a sentence, (2) used more elaborative strategies, and (3) remembered significantly more pairs then when they were presented with congruent noun pairs (e.g., "bush-garden") than incongruent noun pairs (e.g., "glass-elbow").…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…Participants made significantly faster judgments if the noun-name pair was congruent (e.g., engineer -Frank), rather than incongruent (e.g., engineer -Mary), with gender role stereotypes. Likewise, Most et al (2007) reported similar findings using an auditory Stroop task with both adults and children (i.e., 8-to 9-year-olds). In this study, participants heard both male and female voices saying stereotypically masculine or feminine names (e.g., "Amy") or words (e.g., "tough") and were required to rapidly respond as to whether the voice was that of a man or a woman.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 56%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Most people in Euro-American cultures will, for example, think of dogs as somehow masculine and cats as somehow feminine (even though of course both species come in both sexes). Psychological research shows that cognitive gender schemas are important ways in which we "organize incoming information and integrate it-through no conscious act of will-into clusters" (Most, Sorber, & Cunningham, 2007) and that these gender associations pervade perceptions about academic fields (Whitehead, 1996). Very often this dualistic metaphorical structure is also hierarchical.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%