“…For instance, affective priming effects are usually found with short (e.g., 300 ms or shorter) but not with long stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) values (Fazio et al, 1986;Hermans, De Houwer, & Eelen, 2001)-that is, a fast-acting and brief process that does not allow for the activation of expectancies or response strategies. The effect also appears independently of cognitive resources-for instance, when participants are told to perform concurrently a highly demanding secondary task (Hermans, Crombez, & Eelen, 2000); when primes are presented parafoveally in unattended locations (Calvo, Castillo, & Fuentes, 2006;Calvo & Nummenmaa, 2007); or when primes are presented subliminally below individual recognition thresholds (Draine & Greenwald, 1998;Spruyt, De Houwer, Everaert, & Hermans, 2012). Finally, affective priming effects are found even when the task does not induce a conscious intention to evaluate-that is, it does not require evaluative responses over the target (e.g., naming, lexical decision; Bargh, Chaiken, Raymond, & Hymes, 1996;Calvo et al, 2006;Hermans, De Houwer, & Eelen, 1994), although this effect has not been replicated in other studies (e.g., Klauer & Musch, 2001;Spruyt, Hermans, Pandelaere, De Houwer, & Eelen, 2004).…”