2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.10.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Automatic phonological activation during visual word recognition in bilingual children: A cross-language masked priming study in grades 3 and 5

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
5
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
1
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These results are in line with previous studies on cognate facilitation in bilingual children's vocabulary acquisition (Bosma et al, 2019;Comesaña et al, 2012Comesaña et al, , 2019Kelley & Kohnert, 2012;Malabonga et al, 2008;Méndez Pérez et al, 2010;Proctor & Mo, 2009;Schelletter, 2002;Tonzar et al, 2009;Valente et al, 2018), picture naming (Poarch & van Hell, 2012), lexical decision (Brenders et al, 2011;Sauval et al, 2017;Schröter & Schroeder, 2016;Valente et al, 2018), and translation recognition (Duñabeitia et al, 2016) as well as with previous research on cognate facilitation in bilingual adults' sentence reading (Bultena et al, 2014;Duyck et al, 2007;Van Assche et al, 2011. In addition, they support the observation that the degree of cross-language activation depends on children's (reading) proficiency in the non-target language (Brenders et al, 2011;Malabonga et al, 2008;Méndez Pérez et al, 2010).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…These results are in line with previous studies on cognate facilitation in bilingual children's vocabulary acquisition (Bosma et al, 2019;Comesaña et al, 2012Comesaña et al, , 2019Kelley & Kohnert, 2012;Malabonga et al, 2008;Méndez Pérez et al, 2010;Proctor & Mo, 2009;Schelletter, 2002;Tonzar et al, 2009;Valente et al, 2018), picture naming (Poarch & van Hell, 2012), lexical decision (Brenders et al, 2011;Sauval et al, 2017;Schröter & Schroeder, 2016;Valente et al, 2018), and translation recognition (Duñabeitia et al, 2016) as well as with previous research on cognate facilitation in bilingual adults' sentence reading (Bultena et al, 2014;Duyck et al, 2007;Van Assche et al, 2011. In addition, they support the observation that the degree of cross-language activation depends on children's (reading) proficiency in the non-target language (Brenders et al, 2011;Malabonga et al, 2008;Méndez Pérez et al, 2010).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…For children, but not adults, this effect was gradual: they performed better on cognate items with a larger degree of orthographic and phonological overlap between the L1 and the L2 (Valente et al, 2018). In addition, in a lexical decision task with cross-language phonological priming, French word recognition in 8-and 10-year-old early bilinguals was facilitated by the presentation of English phonological primes (Sauval, Perre, Duncan, Marinus, & Casalis, 2017). Furthermore, 8-year-old L1 English children who were enrolled in a French immersion program where they were learning to read simultaneously in English and French made fewer errors on cognate items than on non-cognate items in a French single-word reading-aloud task but not in an English one.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 92%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In a meta-analysis, Ehri and colleagues (2001) demonstrate the importance of early phonological training: children who received training before age seven showed the greatest corresponding benefit in word reading. Rapid, automatic phonological activation appears to be a characteristic of skilled reading, as measured by stronger pseudo-homophone (nonwords, sharing phonology of real words) priming effects among older readers and those with higher scores on standardized reading measures (Booth, Perfetti, & Macwhinney, 1999;Sauval, Perre, Duncan, Marinus, & Casalis, 2017). Skilled readers more rapidly and automatically activate phonological representations from written words .…”
Section: Behavioral Support For the Triangle Modelmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Phonological processing is a fundamental step in lexical access, not only obligatory in oral communication but also involved more than it seems to be in visual word recognition. Although phonological activation in monolingual visual word recognition is well established (see Rastle & Brysbaert, 2006, for a review), and cross-language phonological activation has been obtained among bilinguals with alphabetic languages (e.g., Carrasco-Ortiz et al, 2012; Jouravlev et al, 2014; Sauval et al, 2017), it is less clear whether the activation of phonological representations is language-specific among bilinguals with cross-script language systems during visual word recognition. In particular, little is known about how early automatic phonological activation appears in these bilingual readers and whether this activation changes over time.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%