1959
DOI: 10.1037/h0041312
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Avoidance learning as a function of several training conditions and strain differences in rats.

Abstract: The simplest interpretation of current avoidance learning theories (Solomon & Brush, 1956) might lead one to believe that successful avoidance responding almost inevitably develops if a discriminable cue consistently precedes painful stimulation that motivates vigorous escape responding, and if anticipatory performance of that response can avoid the painful stimulation. As the following study will show, such is hardly the case. Several variables which demand a more adequate theoretical formulation appear to … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3

Citation Types

5
34
2

Year Published

1960
1960
1980
1980

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 71 publications
(41 citation statements)
references
References 5 publications
5
34
2
Order By: Relevance
“…The higher number of avoidances obtained with the acoustic CS may be related to an effectiveness intrinsic to this kind of stimulus in producing avoidance. This has been shown in rats by Myers (1959) who suggests that the startle 150 response elicited by the buzzer serves to antagonize the freezing pattern produced by fear.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…The higher number of avoidances obtained with the acoustic CS may be related to an effectiveness intrinsic to this kind of stimulus in producing avoidance. This has been shown in rats by Myers (1959) who suggests that the startle 150 response elicited by the buzzer serves to antagonize the freezing pattern produced by fear.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…This finding taken together with the high frequency of PLPs under cost on the avoidance-escape schedule suggests the possibility that response cost may be an important factor in the substantial number of Ss who fail to acquire an instrumental avoidance response (Ader and Tatum, 1961) and in the extensive training often required for even ultimately "successful" avoiders (Meyer, Cho, and Wesemann, 1960). It may be, for example, that the frequently observed freezing-crouching patterns of animals, when electric shock is used to shape avoidance responding (Meyers, 1959), adds "physical costs" to responding which interfere with the development of avoidance behavior.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some experimenters, e.g., Schaeffer (1959), reduce the severity of the problem by selecting strains of rats containing a high proportion of animals that learn, others, e.g., D'Amato, Keller, & DiCara (1964) and Baum (1965), manipulate the apparatus and training procedure to favor rapid learning, and still others, e.g., Myers (1959), combine the two approaches.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%