2021
DOI: 10.3758/s13414-021-02280-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Awareness is necessary for attentional biases by location–reward association

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

1
7
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
1
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These findings are consistent with previous studies demonstrating that spatial attentional biases arising from reward learning are contingent upon awareness of the reward contingencies (Mine et al, 2021; Sisk et al, 2020; see also Anderson & Kim, 2018a, 2018b; Liao et al, 2021), which contrasts with the influence of reward learning and aversive conditioning on feature-based attention that can be implicit (Grégoire & Anderson, 2019; Grégoire et al, 2021, 2020; Hopkins et al, 2016; Leganes-Fonteneau et al, 2018, 2019), suggesting a fundamental difference between how selection history shapes feature-based and space-based attentional biases. Like feature-based attentional biases arising from both reward learning and aversive conditioning, however, Pavlovian learning is implicated, which may reflect a broad principle in the selection history-dependent control of attention.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 93%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These findings are consistent with previous studies demonstrating that spatial attentional biases arising from reward learning are contingent upon awareness of the reward contingencies (Mine et al, 2021; Sisk et al, 2020; see also Anderson & Kim, 2018a, 2018b; Liao et al, 2021), which contrasts with the influence of reward learning and aversive conditioning on feature-based attention that can be implicit (Grégoire & Anderson, 2019; Grégoire et al, 2021, 2020; Hopkins et al, 2016; Leganes-Fonteneau et al, 2018, 2019), suggesting a fundamental difference between how selection history shapes feature-based and space-based attentional biases. Like feature-based attentional biases arising from both reward learning and aversive conditioning, however, Pavlovian learning is implicated, which may reflect a broad principle in the selection history-dependent control of attention.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 93%
“…The role of reward learning in the control of spatial attention has also been examined, in situations in which attending to a stimulus only when appearing in a particular spatial location (Bourgeois et al, 2018; Chelazzi et al, 2014; Della Libera et al, 2017; Mine et al, 2021; see also Anderson, 2015) or attending to a particular region of a scene (Anderson & Kim, 2018a, 2018b; Liao et al, 2021) results in high reward. In this case, there is evidence that attention is biased toward reward-associated regions of space (Anderson & Kim, 2018a, 2018b; Bourgeois et al, 2018; Chelazzi et al, 2014; Della Libera et al, 2017; Liao et al, 2021; Mine et al, 2021). Likewise, directional eye movements, divorced from a particular region of a scene, are potentiated when paired with reward (Liao & Anderson, 2020; see also McCoy & Theeuwes, 2018).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A similar proposition has been made for spatial stimulus-reward associations ( Chelazzi et al., 2014 ) but not yet been tested neurally. Recent papers suggest that spatial value-based attentional capture is only successful if participants are aware of the spatial reward contingencies during learning ( Anderson et al., 2021 ; Mine et al., 2021 ). Our study shows that if those contingencies are known, significant changes in neural activity are observed in sensory areas, suggesting that a similar mechanism as observed in feature-based attentional capture is at play under those circumstances.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is manifested behaviorally through faster response times (RTs) in selecting target items that carry reward-associated features ( Kiss et al., 2009 ) and slower RTs when distractor items do so ( Anderson et al., 2016 ). However, behavioral findings on the effects of reward-associated spatial features on attention are conflicting, with some showing that reward-associated locations in our visual field can attract our attention similarly to reward-associated nonspatial features ( Mine et al., 2021 ; Sisk et al., 2020 ; Anderson and Kim, 2018 ; Chelazzi et al., 2014 ), whereas others do not find an effect ( Won and Leber, 2018 ). This discrepancy has been attributed to the role of awareness of participants of the contingencies between spatial locations and reward, with successful spatial bias only occurring when we are explicitly aware of this association ( Anderson et al., 2021 ; Mine et al., 2021 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…This is manifested behaviorally through faster response times (RTs) in selecting target items that carry reward-associated features (Kiss et al, 2009), and slower RTs when distractor items do so (Anderson et al, 2016). However, behavioral findings on the effects of spatial reward-associations on attentional capture are conflicting, with some showing that reward-associated locations in our visual field can capture our attention similarly to reward-associated features (Mine et al, 2021;Sisk et al, 2020;Anderson & Kim, 2018, Chelazzi et al, 2014 whereas others do not find an effect (Won & Leber, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%