2013
DOI: 10.1002/dev.21128
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Baboons' hand preference resists to spatial factors for a communicative gesture but not for a simple manipulative action

Abstract: Olive baboons (Papio anubis) do acquire and use intentional requesting gestures in experimental contexts. Individual's hand preference for these gestures is consistent with that observed for typical communicative gestures, but not for manipulative actions. Here, we examine whether the strength of hand preference may also be a good marker of hemispheric specialization for communicative gestures, hence differing from the strength of hand preference for manipulative actions. We compared the consistency of individ… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
6
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
2
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…When grasping, hand preferences of all the species were shown to depend on item's position, which is in accordance with several studies in monkeys, apes, and humans (Bourjade et al, 2013;Carlson & Harris, 1985;Chapelain et al, 2012;Cronholm, Grodsky, & Behar, 1963;Doyen, Dufour, Caroff, Cherfouh, & Carlier, 2008;Fagard, 1998;Lehman, 1980;Meunier et al, 2011), agreeing that reaching is strongly influenced by situational factors. In other words, humans (infants, children, and adults, e.g., Calvert & Bishop, 1998;Leconte & Fagard, 2006), as well as nonhuman primates, strongly prefer to use the hand that is closest to the item to be reached for in a simple reaching task.…”
Section: Positional Effects Task Nature and Hand Preferencessupporting
confidence: 90%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…When grasping, hand preferences of all the species were shown to depend on item's position, which is in accordance with several studies in monkeys, apes, and humans (Bourjade et al, 2013;Carlson & Harris, 1985;Chapelain et al, 2012;Cronholm, Grodsky, & Behar, 1963;Doyen, Dufour, Caroff, Cherfouh, & Carlier, 2008;Fagard, 1998;Lehman, 1980;Meunier et al, 2011), agreeing that reaching is strongly influenced by situational factors. In other words, humans (infants, children, and adults, e.g., Calvert & Bishop, 1998;Leconte & Fagard, 2006), as well as nonhuman primates, strongly prefer to use the hand that is closest to the item to be reached for in a simple reaching task.…”
Section: Positional Effects Task Nature and Hand Preferencessupporting
confidence: 90%
“…We adapted this task in such a way that it could be used to investigate both human infants and nonhuman primates. Moreover, our adaptation of the QHP task included the design of two experiments to compare both hand preferences while subjects grasped an object and hand preferences while subjects displayed a communicative gesture (see also Bourjade, Meunier, Blois-Heulin, & Vauclair, 2013). One can remark that nonhuman primates do usually not point spontaneously for each other (but see in apes Inoue-Nakamura &Matsuzawa, 1997 andVeà &Sabater-Pi, 1998) and that pointing is a gesture more specific for nonhuman primate-human interactions (e.g., Call & Tomasello, 1994;Mitchell & Anderson, 1997).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, such results are also impressively consistent with the only similar previous study available on hunter‐gatherers (the Hadza, Tanzania, Cavanagh et al, ). In contrast, in non‐human primates and in western human infants (but not in adults, Cochet & Vauclair ), the strength of the hand preference is higher in gestures than in motor actions (e.g., primates: Bourjade et al, ; Hopkins & Leavens, ; Meguerditchian et al, ; infants: Cochet, Jover, & Vauclair, ). However, more surprisingly, we found that nonexplicitly informative gestures were more lateralized than explicitly informative ones.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…We also recorded the position of the target of each behavior (i.e., the position of the objects collected or the localization of the target of the speech, Hopkins & Wesley, 2002; Bryden, Mayer, & Roy, ; Meunier et al, ; Bourjade, Bourjade, Meunier, Blois‐Heulin, & Vauclair, ). The different target positions were defined considering as 0° all the points constituting the bilateral mirror plan: target between ± 10° = “central” position, target between 10°‐180° = “right” and otherwise as “left”‐positioned.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%