2014
DOI: 10.1167/14.1.16
|View full text |Cite|
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Backward position shift in apparent motion

Abstract: We investigated the perceived position of visual targets in apparent motion. A disc moved horizontally through three positions from -10° to +10° in the far periphery (20° above fixation), generating a compelling impression of apparent motion. In the first experiment, observers compared the position of the middle of the three discs to a subsequently presented reference. Unexpectedly, observers judged its position to be shifted backward, in the direction opposite that of the motion. We then tested the middle dis… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Perceived motion is a powerful cue, as it also conveys direction (which is all that is needed in a direction-discrimination task). Further, numerous illusions attest the influence of motion or apparent motion on position estimates [39][40][41]. In the control conditions, those signals are clearly seen and can reinforce the perception of a displacement.…”
Section: A General Bias Revealed Under Conditions Of Impoverished Visionmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Perceived motion is a powerful cue, as it also conveys direction (which is all that is needed in a direction-discrimination task). Further, numerous illusions attest the influence of motion or apparent motion on position estimates [39][40][41]. In the control conditions, those signals are clearly seen and can reinforce the perception of a displacement.…”
Section: A General Bias Revealed Under Conditions Of Impoverished Visionmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Motion-induced position shifts (MIPS) are a class of visual illusion that have long intrigued scientists (e.g., Matin et al, 1976 ; Freyd and Finke, 1984 ; Ramachandran and Inada, 1985 ; Bülthoff et al, 1989 ; Fröhlich, 1923 ; De Valois and De Valois, 1991 ; Nijhawan, 1994 ; Müsseler and Aschersleben, 1998 ; Whitney and Cavanagh, 2000 , 2002 ; Thornton, 2002 ; Müsseler and Kerzel, 2004 ; see Whitney, 2002 ; Burr and Thompson, 2011 for reviews). Today, such effects continue to promote important insights into how motion and position interact during object localization, particularly with respect to the level(s) of processing at which such interactions arise (e.g., Arnold et al, 2007 ; Eagleman and Sejnowski, 2007 ; Mather and Pavan, 2009 ; Shapiro et al, 2010 ; Tse et al, 2011 ; Kosovicheva et al, 2012 ; Maus et al, 2013a , b ; Li et al, 2014 ). The current paper is concerned with one specific visual illusion where local motion within an object causes a shift in its perceived global position (Ramachandran and Anstis, 1990 ; De Valois and De Valois, 1991 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%