Oxford Handbook Topics in Linguistics 2015
DOI: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199935345.013.50
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Bantu Syntax

Abstract: This chapter provides an overview of the common syntactic features as well as the syntactic microvariation found in the Bantu languages. It particularly highlights the importance of information structure for the analysis of morphosyntax in this language family: word order, valency, voice, tense-aspect marking, subject and object marking can all be influenced and affected by the information structure expressed in the sentence. The chapter furthermore shows how Bantu languages, despite their shared basic SVO wor… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…traditional abstract Case or what we have referred to as Vergnaud licensing) and search for an alternative nominal licensing system in these languages. Since information structure is known to play a pervasive role in the syntax of Bantu languages (Downing & Hyman 2015, Van der Wal 2015c, Güldemann, Zerbian & Zimmermann 2015), the alternative licensing system might be hypothesised to be based upon one or more categories in information structure. For example, argument indexing on the verb and movement to the preverbal position need not be related to subject licensing: The preverbal position in many Bantu languages has been argued to be restricted to topical/non-focal elements (see, among others, Bokamba 1976; Morimoto 2000; Sabel & Zeller 2006; Zerbian 2006, 2007; Van der Wal 2009; Yoneda 2011), and ‘subject’ agreement has been argued to be directly related to topicality (Morimoto 2006 for Kirundi), or indirectly via the requirement to agree with a higher, preverbal element (Baker 2003, 2008; Collins 2004; Carstens 2005; Diercks 2011).…”
Section: Summary Of Results and Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…traditional abstract Case or what we have referred to as Vergnaud licensing) and search for an alternative nominal licensing system in these languages. Since information structure is known to play a pervasive role in the syntax of Bantu languages (Downing & Hyman 2015, Van der Wal 2015c, Güldemann, Zerbian & Zimmermann 2015), the alternative licensing system might be hypothesised to be based upon one or more categories in information structure. For example, argument indexing on the verb and movement to the preverbal position need not be related to subject licensing: The preverbal position in many Bantu languages has been argued to be restricted to topical/non-focal elements (see, among others, Bokamba 1976; Morimoto 2000; Sabel & Zeller 2006; Zerbian 2006, 2007; Van der Wal 2009; Yoneda 2011), and ‘subject’ agreement has been argued to be directly related to topicality (Morimoto 2006 for Kirundi), or indirectly via the requirement to agree with a higher, preverbal element (Baker 2003, 2008; Collins 2004; Carstens 2005; Diercks 2011).…”
Section: Summary Of Results and Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Notice, the order √ -Y -X is not predicted to be possible to derive by head movement given the Mirror Principle and the Head Movement Constraint (Travis 1984). That is to say the closer head to √ cannot be separated from it by a head further away.…”
Section: … Y X √mentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Hence, c-command is translated into linear precedence by Merger. Head Movement, which I assume is subject to the Head Movement Constraint (Travis 1984) can bring heads into configurations allowing Merger. Under Kayne's (1994: 15-21) approach, the moved head c-commands the head it adjoins to and thus syntactic head movement always results in left-adjunction.…”
Section: … Y X √mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Verbs in Kinyarwanda are highly inflectional, and are commonly analyzed as consisting of a string of morphemes, the order of which is determined by a template (Banerjee 2019;Creissels 2019;Hyman 2003;Hyman & Inkelas 2017;Wal 2015). In his analysis of Kinyarwanda, Banerjee (2019) follows most literature on Bantu (Hyman 2003;Hyman & Inkelas 2017;Wal 2015) and specifies that this template determines the order of the morphemes as follows: subject -tense -object -verbal radical -extensions -aspect -final vowel. Extension is a collective term for a number of valency changing morphemes (Banerjee 2019), and the final vowel is sometimes analyzed as a mood marker (Goldsmith & Mpiranya 2010).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In Proto-Bantu the noun class system was semantically productive, but synchronically it no longer is. For each noun there is a class for the singular and a different one for the plural (Demuth 2000;Nurse & Philippson 2006;Wal 2015). For example the Kinyarwanda noun umupfayongo "absent minded person" is of class one and can be represented as follows: the lexeme pfayongo and its morpholosyntactic property set {class one}: <pfayongo, {class one}>.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%