2000
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.2000.74-331
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Behavior Analysis and Revaluation

Abstract: Revaluation refers to phenomena in which the strength of an operant is altered by reinforcer-related manipulations that take place outside the conditioning situation in which the operant was selected. As an example, if lever pressing is acquired using food as a reinforcer and food is later paired with an aversive stimulus, the frequency of lever pressing decreases when subsequently tested. Associationist psychology infers from such findings that conditioning produces a response-outcome (i.e., reinforcer) assoc… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
12
0
3

Year Published

2004
2004
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
4

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 80 publications
0
12
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…[8], [27], [28], [29], [30], [31]). Therefore, lever pressing by both of the 10S10E groups during the extinction test likely was governed not just by 10S10E-reinforced learning, but also by 10S-reinforced learning.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…[8], [27], [28], [29], [30], [31]). Therefore, lever pressing by both of the 10S10E groups during the extinction test likely was governed not just by 10S10E-reinforced learning, but also by 10S-reinforced learning.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It has provided a wealth of useful information about the speed with which discriminations are learned, the ability of learned performances to survive delays between the discriminative stimuli and the opportunity to respond, transfer of performance across disparate stimuli, and the associative relations to which subjects are sensitive and that underlie these behavioral effects. Differentialoutcome data have contributed substantially to theoretical analyses of discrimination learning, in particular, and instrumental learning, in general (e.g., Colwill, 1994;Colwill & Rescorla, 1986;Urcuioli & DeMarse, 1996), and to the understanding of diverse issues, such as the nature of working memory (e.g., Honig & Dodd, 1986;Overmier, Savage, & Sweeney, 1999;Urcuioli & Zentall, 1992), the origins of equivalence classes (e.g., Astley & Wasserman, 1999;de Rose, McIlvane, Dube, Galpin, & Stoddard, 1988;Edwards, Jagielo, Zentall, & Hogan, 1982), the neural bases of associative learning (e.g., Blundell, Hall, & Killcross, 2001;Savage, 2001;Savage & Parsons, 1997; see also Donahoe & Burgos, 2000), and behavioral remediation (e.g., Estévez, Fuentes, Overmier, & González, 2003;Hochhalter & Joseph, 2001;Malanga & Poling, 1992;Overmier et al, 1999).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…More recently, Burgos and Murillo-Rodríguez [73] used a neural-network model to simulate two context-dependent phenomena in Pavlovian conditioning: context specificity and renewal. Prior to that, the computational framework was used to simulate a wide range of conditioning phenomena such as reacquisition savings [74], reinforcement reevaluation [75], superstition [76], and latent inhibition [77]. Although these neural-network models were inspired by biological data of Pavlovian fear conditioning, there was no correspondence between the models and exact neural structures, and the amygdala circuit was not modeled explicitly.…”
Section: Connectionist Modelsmentioning
confidence: 99%