1983
DOI: 10.1016/s0047-2484(83)80023-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Behavioral processes leading to linear status hierarchies following group formation in rhesus monkeys

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
18
0

Year Published

1987
1987
2004
2004

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 30 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
0
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Our results here show that these processes are at the core of the as-yet-unanswered riddle of hierarchy formation. Empirical and theoretical work on winner, loser, and bystander effects (25)(26)(27)(28)(29)(39)(40)(41)(42) and applications of Chase's jigsaw puzzle model (5,23,(35)(36)(37)(38) seem to be very promising starting points. Both of these lines of research suggest that linear structure is promoted by positive feedback to initial wins and losses during hierarchy formation, i.e., when an animal dominating in one contest goes on to dominate in others and when an animal becoming subordinate in one contest goes on to be subordinate in others.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Our results here show that these processes are at the core of the as-yet-unanswered riddle of hierarchy formation. Empirical and theoretical work on winner, loser, and bystander effects (25)(26)(27)(28)(29)(39)(40)(41)(42) and applications of Chase's jigsaw puzzle model (5,23,(35)(36)(37)(38) seem to be very promising starting points. Both of these lines of research suggest that linear structure is promoted by positive feedback to initial wins and losses during hierarchy formation, i.e., when an animal dominating in one contest goes on to dominate in others and when an animal becoming subordinate in one contest goes on to be subordinate in others.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some of these sequences ensured the development of transitive dominance relationships and thus the efficient production of linear hierarchies, whereas others led to either transitive or intransitive relationships and thus, possibly, nonlinear hierarchies. Researchers applying the model have found high proportions of the sequences ensuring transitivity in a range of species: chickens, rhesus monkeys, Japanese macaques, cichlid fish, and crayfish (4,5,23,(35)(36)(37)(38). Winner, loser, and bystander effects may account for the high proportion of sequences ensuring transitive relationships, and researchers using mathematical models and computer simulation have demonstrated that if these effects occur during hierarchy formation, they can enhance the production of linear structures even when all individuals are identical initially in prior attributes (39)(40)(41)(42).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Many experimental studies that use non-human primates as animal models use body mass as a covariate or an outcome variable: for example, growth studies [Glassman et al, 1984], studies of reproduction , hypertension [Kammerer, 1995], obesity [Wolden-Hanson et al, 1993], social dominance relationships [Mendoza & Barchas, 1983], etc. Ignoring such a sizable genetic contribution to body mass can produce inflated estimates of the statistical significance of associations between experimental factors and weight [Williams-Blangero, 1993].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Such 'loser and winner effects' (Chase, 1982a(Chase, ,b, 1985(Chase, , 1986Chase and Rohwer, 1987), in which an individual which is observed to dominate in one contest is more likely to be observed to dominate in a subsequent contest, have been reported in chickens (McBride, 1958;Chase, 1980Chase, , 1982aChase, ,b, 1985, crickets (Alexander, 1961;Burk, 1979), fish (Francis, 1983;Beaugrand and Zayan, 1984), mice (Ginsburg and Allee, 1975), rats (Van de Poll et al, 1982), rhesus monkeys (Mendoza and Barchas, 1983;Barchas and Mendoza, 1984), bumblebees (Van Honk and Hogeweg, 1981), wasps (Theraulaz et al, 1989(Theraulaz et al, , 1992, and more recently crayfish (Gössmann and Huber, 1999).…”
Section: Modelsmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…When such a network of dominance-submission relationships, a hierarchy, arises in a stable group, it organizes the group in such a way that conflicts do not completely offset the advantages of group living. Dominance behavior has been described in hens [e.g., Schjelderup-Ebbe (1913, 1922; Allee (1942Allee ( , 1951Allee ( , 1952; Guhl (1968)], cows [e.g., Schein and Fohrman (1955); Barton et al (1974)], ponies [e.g., Tyler (1972)], fish [e.g., Lowe (1956); Bovbjerg (1956); Bovbjerg and Stephen (1971); Wilson (1975)], in crabs, lobsters, and crayfish (Jachowsky, 1974;Glass and Huntingford, 1988;Huber and Kravitz, 1995), lizards Evans (1951Evans ( , 1953, frogs when they are crowded together [e.g., Haubrich (1961); Boice and Witter (1969)], rats [e.g., Van de Poll et al (1982)], primates [e.g., Kummer (1968); Baldwin (1971); Candland and Leshner (1971); Mendoza and Barchas (1983); Thierry (1985)], or social insects Wilson (1971), especially in wasps [e.g., Gervet (1962Gervet ( , 1964; Pardi (1942Pardi ( , 1946Pardi ( , 1948; West Eberhard (1969); Evans and Eberhard (1970); Röseler et al (1986); Röseler (1991); Theraulaz et al (1992)], ants [e.g., Cole (1981); …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%