1974
DOI: 10.1126/science.185.4154.824
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Behavioral Regulation of the Milieu Interne in Man and Rat

Abstract: In regulating the internal homeostatic environment mammals, by necessity, employ behavioral strategies that differ from the tactics used in coping with contingencies in the external environment. When an animal consumes a meal, the palatability of that meal is automatically adjusted in accordance with the ultimate internal effects of that meal. If the meal causes toxicosis, the animal acquires an aversion for the taste of the meal; conversely, if recuperation follows ingestion of the meal, the taste of that mea… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

11
435
0
5

Year Published

1982
1982
2012
2012

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 1,096 publications
(451 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
11
435
0
5
Order By: Relevance
“…A conditioned stimulus (CS) is associated with an unconditioned stimulus (US). In the case of conditioned food preference and aversion, animals come to consume or avoid a food (CS) that produces positive or negative post-ingestive symptoms (US), respectively (Pavlov, 1960;Garcia et al, 1974).…”
Section: Characteristics and Development Of Preferences And Aversionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…A conditioned stimulus (CS) is associated with an unconditioned stimulus (US). In the case of conditioned food preference and aversion, animals come to consume or avoid a food (CS) that produces positive or negative post-ingestive symptoms (US), respectively (Pavlov, 1960;Garcia et al, 1974).…”
Section: Characteristics and Development Of Preferences And Aversionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This ability to learn to avoid potentially toxic foods has been demonstrated in numerous animal species, from invertebrate to humans (for a review, see Bernstein, 1999;Paradis and Cabanac, 2004). Indeed, food aversion has been described in a variety of mammals, in addition to humans (Garcia et al, 1974; Bellisle, 1999;Ravasco, 2005; Bellisle, 2006) or rats (Yasoshima et al, 2000;Ferreira, 2004), and in livestock species such as horses, sheep and cattle (Houpt et al, 1990;Burritt and Provenza, 1996;Halaweish et al, 2002;Ginane and Dumont, 2006;Pfister et al, 2007), and also in birds (Skelhorn and Rowe, 2006;Halpin et al, 2008;Skelhorn et al, 2008) and reptile species (Terrick et al, 1995;Paradis and Cabanac, 2004). Experimentally induced food aversions are frequently conducted by an intragastric or an intraperitoneal injection of lithium chloride, an emetic substance known to induce visceral malaise.…”
Section: Characteristics and Development Of Preferences And Aversionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Conditioned taste preferences and conditioned taste aversions provide strong proof that responses to food cues can be learned and modified. For example, most animals readily learn to avoid foods that render them ill; a special visceral learning linked to food ingestion (Garcia et al, 1974;Rozin, 1976). Bait shyness linked to poison is well known and is an important adaptation (Richter, 1953).…”
Section: Evidence For Central Nervous System Contributionmentioning
confidence: 99%