1998
DOI: 10.1002/(sici)1099-0992(1998110)28:6<861::aid-ejsp899>3.0.co;2-d
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Behavioral routines in decision making: the effects of novelty in task presentation and time pressure on routine maintenance and deviation

Abstract: This article examines the role of behavioral routines in decision making. In order to induce routines, participants were confronted with recurrent route decisions in a computer-controlled trucking game, which allows for manipulation of routine acquisition and strength. During the ®nal round of the game, time pressure and novelty in task presentation were varied as between-factors. It was hypothesized that time pressure would increase the likelihood of routine maintenance and novelty would increase the likeliho… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
41
0

Year Published

2001
2001
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 76 publications
(42 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
1
41
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Alternatively, people act automatically (e.g., Bargh, Chen, & Burrows, 1996;Jonas & Sassenberg, 2006), based on habits (e.g., Aarts & Dijksterhuis, 2000;Verplanken & Aarts, 1999;Verplanken, Aarts, van Knippenberg, & van Knippenberg, 1994), or initiated by non-conscious goals and implicit motivation (Custers & Aarts, 2005;Veltkamp, Aarts, & Custers, 2009). Not surprisingly, people are especially likely to resort to routines or habits under suboptimal conditions (Betsch, Fiedler, & Brinkmann, 1998).…”
Section: The Cognitive Miser In Social Psychologymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Alternatively, people act automatically (e.g., Bargh, Chen, & Burrows, 1996;Jonas & Sassenberg, 2006), based on habits (e.g., Aarts & Dijksterhuis, 2000;Verplanken & Aarts, 1999;Verplanken, Aarts, van Knippenberg, & van Knippenberg, 1994), or initiated by non-conscious goals and implicit motivation (Custers & Aarts, 2005;Veltkamp, Aarts, & Custers, 2009). Not surprisingly, people are especially likely to resort to routines or habits under suboptimal conditions (Betsch, Fiedler, & Brinkmann, 1998).…”
Section: The Cognitive Miser In Social Psychologymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, observers might consider the hypothesis that the situation is novel ("world has changed"). Perceived novelty is one important factor fostering routine deviation (Betsch et al, 1998(Betsch et al, , 2001. Discrepant behaviors in models may serve as a cue for novelty.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Betsch et al, 1998). Six participants failed to gain 250 points within 100 trials (see result section).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Thus, routines and habits may seem very similar. According to Betsch, Fiedler, and Brinkmann (1998), a routine is "a behavioral option that comes to mind as a solution when the decision-maker is confronted with a certain decision problem" (p. 862), while a habit is considered as a special case of routine, given that routines do not necessarily require a personal or repetitive experience of the behaviors Routines and habits are necessary to good adaptation, and the absence of habits requires controlled choices and behaviors (Verplanken, 2006). A major characteristic of habits is that their occurrence is automatized, so they can easily occur in parallel with other behaviors (Aarts & Dijksterhuis, 2000).…”
Section: Routinization and Agingmentioning
confidence: 99%