In a fairly large number of studies, researchers have examined the effects of extinctionon conditionedresponding in classical and operant conditioningprocedures. Neuringer, Kornell, and Olufs (2001) found evidence in this literature, which will not be reviewed here again, of two opposing effects: response stability and response variability. An example of each of these effects can be found in the experiments reported by Neuringer et al. (2001). In their first experiment, rats were food-reinforced for emitting variable sequencesof responses, using one, two, or three of three different operanda: two levers (L and R) and a key (K). Each response sequence that could be reinforced consisted of three responses. There are a total of 27 possible threeresponse sequences involvingone, two, or three operand(a) (e.g., LLL, LLR, or LRK). Reinforcement was given for a sequence only if its relative frequency was less than or equal to .05. The purpose of this reinforcement schedule was to induce behavioral variability, and the question of major interest was whether, and how, response variability would change in a subsequent phase without reinforcement (extinction). The authors found evidence of behavioral stability, in that the ordering of the probabilities of the 27 possible sequences during extinction did not differ much from the ordering observed during reinforcement. The most preferred sequences during reinforcement were still preferred the most during extinction; the same was true for the least preferred sequences. However, interestingly, at the same time, the relative frequencies, expressed as a ratio that was computed by dividing the probability during extinction by the probability during reinforcement, of the least preferred sequences increased. This is indicative of increased response variability. This mix of stability and variability was also obtained in two further experiments in which extinctionwas preceded either by responseindependentnoncontingentreinforcement or by consistent reinforcement of only one single sequence. Neuringer et al. (2001) argued that the mix of stability and variability makes sense from an evolutionary perspective. On the one hand, stability implies continuationof behavior that was successful in the past; on the other hand, variability ensures the occurrence of (relatively) new behavior that, potentially, may be more successful than previous behavior.The aim of the present experiments was to evaluate the balance between response stability and variability in humans, rather than in rats, and to examine more than one type of contingency change. As will be outlined below, the type of contingency transition may be expected to be important with respect to the extent of behavioral stability and variability. More generally, data on the conditions that are or are not conducive to the occurrence of behavioral stability and variability (adaptive or not) may be relevant to applied settings, such as educational and therapeutic contexts.In the present experiments, I used a task that was modeled after that em...