1980
DOI: 10.1016/0091-3057(80)90010-6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Behavioural and endocrine effects of naltrexone in male talapoin monkeys

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
30
1

Year Published

1987
1987
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 83 publications
(32 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
1
30
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In obtaining more grooming, the behavior of the naloxone-treated, adult rhesus females in this study was like that of talapoin monkeys (Fabre-Nys et al, 1982;Keverne et al, 1989;Meller et al, 1980), but unlike that of adult lactating rhesus females that had small (4-to 10-week-old) infants, and actually showed a decrease in grooming interactions after naloxone treatment (Martel et al, 1993). Mothers are very protective of their new offspring and will often avoid companions that are curious about the new arrival and may try to touch, hold, or even kidnap the infant (Hinde & Procter, 1977;Maestripieri, 1993;Rowell, Hinde, & Spencer-Booth, 1964).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…In obtaining more grooming, the behavior of the naloxone-treated, adult rhesus females in this study was like that of talapoin monkeys (Fabre-Nys et al, 1982;Keverne et al, 1989;Meller et al, 1980), but unlike that of adult lactating rhesus females that had small (4-to 10-week-old) infants, and actually showed a decrease in grooming interactions after naloxone treatment (Martel et al, 1993). Mothers are very protective of their new offspring and will often avoid companions that are curious about the new arrival and may try to touch, hold, or even kidnap the infant (Hinde & Procter, 1977;Maestripieri, 1993;Rowell, Hinde, & Spencer-Booth, 1964).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…However, this hypothesis does not satisfactorily explain grooming on body areas accessible to the beneficiary [7], [8]. In addition, grooming appears to increase psychological and physiological well-being through the release of β -endorphine [9], [10] and the decrease in heart rate [11], [12]. Furthermore, social functions of grooming have been widely suggested as a way to establish harmonious relationships between group members and for the maintenance of social affinity [13], [14].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition to oxytocin, a number of studies of primates and other mammals have implicated endogenous opioids in the development of the maternal and social bond and in the mediation of separation-induced distress [16,40,41,44,71,72,74,76,87]. In particular, it has been suggested that the activation of the û-endorphin system during late pregnancy and suckling may promote the positive affect arising from maternal bonds, whereas the pharmacological reduction of opioid activity may promote a central state comparable to that occurring when the safety of the offspring is threatened [76].…”
Section: Neuroendocrine Regulation Of Maternal Responsivenessmentioning
confidence: 99%