2016
DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00536
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Berlyne Revisited: Evidence for the Multifaceted Nature of Hedonic Tone in the Appreciation of Paintings and Music

Abstract: In his seminal book on esthetics, Berlyne (1971) posited an inverted-U relationship between complexity and hedonic tone in arts appreciation, however, converging evidence for his theory is still missing. The disregard of the multidimensionality of complexity may explain some of the divergent results. Here, we argue that definitions of hedonic tone are manifold and systematically examined whether the nature of the relationship between complexity and hedonic tone is determined by the specific measure of hedonic … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

10
63
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 80 publications
(74 citation statements)
references
References 97 publications
10
63
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Pleasantness ratings were found to be less internally consistent than most of our other measures, which is in line with some previous work (Marin, Lampatz, Wandl, & Leder, 2016).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…Pleasantness ratings were found to be less internally consistent than most of our other measures, which is in line with some previous work (Marin, Lampatz, Wandl, & Leder, 2016).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…Liking and beauty ratings may be a complex context-dependent function of many variables, and so may be EADs (Güçlütürk et al, 2016; Marin et al, 2016), but perhaps in the end it always involves implicit associations with joyful or disgusting events triggered by the stimulus, the former taking a greater weight in the decision. Future studies on EADs should thus pay more attention to the underlying (asymmetric) affective-semantic structure (i.e., relating to discrete or embodied emotions) of the stimuli used, before a priori polarizing them symmetrically into “positive” or “negative” ones.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To say it in the words of Silvia (2009, p. 48): “The psychology of aesthetic experience is eerily close to the psychology of how much novices say they like something.” However, the heterogeneity of notions, e.g., aesthetic experience, aesthetic episode, aesthetic appraisal, aesthetic judgment, aesthetic feeling/emotion, aesthetic evaluation etc., methods and materials used to assess felt beauty make it difficult to draw more precise conclusions at this stage (cf. Güçlütürk et al, 2016; Marin et al, 2016; Pelowski et al, 2016). In the following sections, we examine methods used to measure EADs in the context of verbal materials of differing degrees of complexity.…”
Section: The Role Of Valence and Beauty In Theories Of Emotion Aesthmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Many studies have observed a relationship between visual complexity and affect (e.g., pleasantness). This relationship has been observed dating back to the early 1970s (e.g., Kaplan et al, 1972 ; Aitken, 1974 ; Aitken and Hutt, 1974 ) and this idea has re-emerged more recently (e.g., Stamps, 2002 ; Marin and Leder, 2013 , 2016 ; Schlochtermeier et al, 2013 ; Machado et al, 2015 ; Marin et al, 2016 ). Importantly, these studies suggest that more complex pictures are perceived as more pleasant than less complex pictures, a hypothesis supported by earlier work where pleasantness and physiological arousal have been found to be higher for more complex abstract shapes (e.g., Berlyne et al, 1963 , 1968 ; Vitz, 1964 ; Day, 1967 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 91%