2014
DOI: 10.1177/1362361313516547
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Better to be equal? Challenges to equality for cognitively able children with autism spectrum disorders in a social decision game

Abstract: Much controversy surrounds questions about whether humans have an aversion to inequity and how a commitment to equality might play a role in cooperation and other aspects of social interactions. Examining the social decisions of children with autism spectrum disorders provides a fascinating opportunity to explore these issues. Specifically, we evaluated the possibility that children with autism spectrum disorders may be less likely than typically developing children to show a prioritisation of equality. A tota… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

5
24
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
5
24
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In particular, although both ASD and TD might sustain an innate desire for their material possessions, their decision-making during the UG may not entirely align with the economic model of rational pursuit of self-interest 64 , which perhaps could be associated with ASDs’ atypical egocentrism (e.g. restricted or stereotyped behaviour) and self-other reciprocity 52 , 53 , as well as altered reasoning and emotional processing 8 , 65 . Thus, it is possible that the response in the UG among ASD might have also measured some other factors (including the above) rather than a sole flexibility.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In particular, although both ASD and TD might sustain an innate desire for their material possessions, their decision-making during the UG may not entirely align with the economic model of rational pursuit of self-interest 64 , which perhaps could be associated with ASDs’ atypical egocentrism (e.g. restricted or stereotyped behaviour) and self-other reciprocity 52 , 53 , as well as altered reasoning and emotional processing 8 , 65 . Thus, it is possible that the response in the UG among ASD might have also measured some other factors (including the above) rather than a sole flexibility.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is imperative to further assess the flexibility regarding the fairness-preference and perspective-taking (emotional understanding of others) as these emotional processing could be altered in ASD 1 . Likewise, it is also essential to investigate the potential presence of a gap between the level of perceived inequality aversion and rule-based fairness preference towards others in the ASD population 52 , 67 . In this relation, a more robust validation of DFlex in people with ASD is warranted, although DFlex has been applied in several studies investigating autism.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Each subtype of pro-social behavior has unique characteristics and developmental trajectories, and thus it is important to consider each individually when it comes to the behavior of autistic children and adolescents [Dunfield & Kuhlmeier, 2013]. In terms of sharing behavior, despite an abundance of anecdotal data indicating a deficit and many interventions focused on increasing sharing in autistic children [Lane & Ledford, 2016], when presented with resource allocation tasks they show similar sharing tendencies to their non-autistic peers [Sally & Hill, 2006;Schmitz, Banerjee, Pouw, Stockmann, & Rieffe, 2015;Hartley & Fisher, 2018] and have even been shown to share more resources than their peers [Paulus & Rosal-Grifoll, 2017]. With regard to comforting, although autistic children express less facial concern [Butean, Costescu, & Dobrean, 2014], they engage in equal amounts of verbal and nonverbal comforting as their non-autistic peers when someone expresses physical pain [Butean et al, 2014;Travis, Sigman, & Ruskin, 2001;Bacon, Fein, Morris, Waterhouse, & Allen, 1998] or appears visibly upset when excluded from a ball-tossing game [Deschamps, Been, & Matthys, 2014].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…of tasks adapted from those used in economic decisionmaking games, including simplified versions of the dictator and ultimatum games (e.g., Benenson et al 2007;Blake and Rand 2010;Gummerum et al 2008Gummerum et al , 2010Harbaugh and Krause 2000), and the prosocial choice test (e.g., Brownell et al 2009;Claidiere et al 2015;Dahlman et al 2007;Fehr et al 2008;House et al 2012House et al , 2013Schmitz et al 2015). The most common, although not exclusive (e.g., House et al 2012), developmental pattern witnessed across tasks is one in which children become increasingly generous with age (e.g., Benenson et al 2007;Blake and Rand 2010;Brownell et al 2009;Gummerum et al 2010), a pattern that is most evident in situations where the donor pays no direct or relative cost for their generosity (e.g., Dahlman et al 2007;Fehr et al 2008;Thompson et al 1997).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%