2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.10.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Beyond associations: Sensitivity to structure in pre-schoolers’ linguistic predictions

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
25
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 36 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
1
25
1
Order By: Relevance
“…However, it may also be that adults’ ability to adapt to the current linguistic environment is a consequence of language learning occurring through prediction and error minimization. In this vein, several researchers have proposed that prediction is important for language acquisition (e.g., Chang, Dell, & Bock, ; Gambi, Pickering, & Rabagliati, ; Ylinen, Bosseler, Junttila, & Huotilainen, ). To explore this question, we examine whether 3‐ to 4‐year‐old children draw on recently encountered information to revise their predictions about the likely syntactic category of upcoming words, and exploit these revised predictions to infer novel words’ meanings.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, it may also be that adults’ ability to adapt to the current linguistic environment is a consequence of language learning occurring through prediction and error minimization. In this vein, several researchers have proposed that prediction is important for language acquisition (e.g., Chang, Dell, & Bock, ; Gambi, Pickering, & Rabagliati, ; Ylinen, Bosseler, Junttila, & Huotilainen, ). To explore this question, we examine whether 3‐ to 4‐year‐old children draw on recently encountered information to revise their predictions about the likely syntactic category of upcoming words, and exploit these revised predictions to infer novel words’ meanings.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Visual-world eye tracking has not been previously used to study schizophrenia, yet it is particularly well suited for this purpose as it provides a naturalistic and minimally demanding experimental analogue to everyday communication. In our paradigm, participants interacted with a set of real-world objects placed in front of them (following Keysar, Barr, Balin, & Brauner, 2000; Sedivy, Tanenhaus, Chambers, & Carlson, 1999; Tanenhaus et al, 1995; and see also Diehl, Friedberg, Paul, & Snedeker, 2015; Gambi, Pickering, & Rabagliati, 2016; Huang & Snedeker, 2009a, 2009b; Snedeker & Trueswell, 2004; Snedeker & Yuan, 2008; Trueswell, Sekerina, Hill, & Logrip, 1999 for work validating this paradigm in populations other than typical adults). For example, participants might see (1) a toy frog holding a small feather, (2) a large feather, (3) a toy cat holding a small flower, and (4) a large flower (see Figure 1).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We adopted the growth curve analysis (GCA), a multilevel regression modeling technique using fourth-order orthogonal polynomials (Mirman et al, 2008), to quantify the effects of semantic relations. Four terms are included in such analyses, the intercept term represents the mean proportion of fixations over the entire window, the linear term captures variation in how rapidly looks to an object rise over time, the quadratic term captures variation in the curvature of the line representing looks to each object, and the cubic and quartic terms reflect the inflections at the extremities of the curve (Kalénine et al, 2012; Gambi et al, 2016). All analyses were carried out in R version 3.3.2 using the lme4 package (version 1.1-17).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%