“…Such students are often termed “high‐functioning dyslexics” (e.g., Gallagher, Laxon, Armstrong, & Frith, ; Pedersen, Fusaroli, Lauridsen, & Parrila, ) because they have succeeded in being enrolled in educational institutions assumed to require good literacy skills, such as universities (e.g., Hatcher, Snowling, & Griffiths, ; Kirby, Silvestri, Allingham, Parrila, & La Fave, ). Studies of such students have shown, in line with the phonological deficit theory of dyslexia (Lyon, Shaywitz, & Shaywitz, ), that many high‐functioning dyslexic students experience persistent problems in phonological awareness (e.g., Gallagher et al, ; Hatcher et al, ; Parrila, Georgiou, & Corkett, ; Snowling, Nation, Moxham, Gallagher, & Frith, ), nonword reading (e.g., Re, Tressoldi, Cornoldi, & Lucangeli, ; Tops, Callens, Lammertyn, Van Hees, & Brysbaert, ), and spelling (e.g., Kemp, Parrila, & Kirby, ; Snowling et al, ; Tops, Callens, Van Cauwenberghe, Adriaens, & Brysbaert, ). In terms of reading, most studies have found word‐reading deficits and reading‐speed deficits to persist (e.g., Deacon, Cook, & Parrila, ; Re et al, ; Snowling et al, ), with reading comprehension assessments showing more variable results (e.g., Deacon et al, ; Re et al, ).…”