2022
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105245
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Biased confabulation in risky choice

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

3
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 70 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Specifically, people are more likely to report the extremes of the experienced range as the first outcome to come to mind on recall tests, and they overestimate the frequency of extreme outcomes (best and worst) relative to equally often experienced nonextreme outcomes (Madan et al, 2014(Madan et al, , 2017. These effects in memory for reward are typically strongest and most consistent for the worst outcomes (i.e., relative losses; see Ludvig et al, 2015;Madan et al, 2019;Mason et al, 2022). For risky effort, it appears that people are also most likely to overweight the worst outcome, but in this case the worst outcome is the one requiring the most effort (highest number of clicks).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, people are more likely to report the extremes of the experienced range as the first outcome to come to mind on recall tests, and they overestimate the frequency of extreme outcomes (best and worst) relative to equally often experienced nonextreme outcomes (Madan et al, 2014(Madan et al, , 2017. These effects in memory for reward are typically strongest and most consistent for the worst outcomes (i.e., relative losses; see Ludvig et al, 2015;Madan et al, 2019;Mason et al, 2022). For risky effort, it appears that people are also most likely to overweight the worst outcome, but in this case the worst outcome is the one requiring the most effort (highest number of clicks).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our study has shown that memory is reconstructive by creating new yet false associations. A recent study also found that people may not exhibit veridical recall but confabulate rewarded outcomes in experienced-based decision tasks (Mason et al, 2022). One might ask, why would our memory spontaneously construct false associations involving novel stimuli?…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In experience-based decision making, people’s memories of past rewarding outcomes tend to be biased in a way that they normally overestimate the extreme gains and losses, and such memory bias is found to be correlated with risk preferences (Ludvig et al, 2014; Madan et al, 2014; Mason et al, 2022). This implies that what people remember about reward outcomes is not always what they have truly experienced, suggesting the potential role of distorted memory in decision making.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The formation of beliefs and preferences is closely tied to these memory biases. As we gather experiences, we develop decision-making heuristics that guide our choices in various situations (Madan et al, 2021; Mason et al, 2022; Weber & Johnson, 2006). These heuristics are shaped not only by direct memories of past events but also by experiences that influence our beliefs and behaviors, even if we are not consciously able to recollect them (Addis & Tippett, 2004; Guzmán-Vélez et al, 2014; Kunst-Wilson & Zajonc, 1980; Madan, 2020, 2023; Palombo et al, 2021).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%