2018
DOI: 10.31219/osf.io/tsr36
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Bilingual Experience and Executive Control over the Adult Lifespan: The Role of Biological Sex

Abstract: Cite this article as: Subramaniapillai, S., Rajah, M.N., Pasvanis, S., Titone, D. (2018 We investigated whether bilingual language experience over the lifespan impacts women and men in a manner that differentially buffers against age-related declines in executive control.To this end, we investigated whether executive control performance in a lifespan sample of adult women and men were differentially impacted by individual differences in bilingual language experience, assessed using an unspeeded measure of exec… Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

6
0

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We extracted classic measures of L2 exposure, such as L2 AoA (based on the onset of learning) and global exposure to the L1, L2, and L3 (third language). Global L2 exposure is frequently used as a covariate in the MLL lab (e.g., Pivneva et al, 2014; Subramaniapillai et al, 2018), and these global exposure measures did not factor in to the computation of language diversity to allow for a comparison of the measures.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We extracted classic measures of L2 exposure, such as L2 AoA (based on the onset of learning) and global exposure to the L1, L2, and L3 (third language). Global L2 exposure is frequently used as a covariate in the MLL lab (e.g., Pivneva et al, 2014; Subramaniapillai et al, 2018), and these global exposure measures did not factor in to the computation of language diversity to allow for a comparison of the measures.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Bilingual and multilingual individuals vary widely in their exposure to, and socially distributed use of, language, particularly if they live in highly multilingual locations. Individual differences in static language experiences, such as age of language acquisition (AoA; e.g., Flege, Munro & MacKay, 1995; Gullifer, Chai, Whitford, Pivneva, Baum, Klein & Titone, 2018; Kousaie, Chai, Sander & Klein, 2017; Luk, De Sa & Bialystok, 2011; Piske, MacKay & Flege, 2001; Subramaniapillai, Rajah, Pasvanis & Titone, 2018; Titone, Libben, Mercier, Whitford & Pivneva, 2011), and current language experiences, such as amount of second language (L2) exposure (e.g., Gullifer et al, 2018; Hartanto & Yang, 2016; Hofweber, Marinis & Treffers-Daller, 2016; Jylkkä, Soveri, Wahlström, Lehtonen, Rodriguez-Fornells & Laine, 2017; Pivneva, Mercier & Titone, 2014; Prior & Gollan, 2011; Subramaniapillai et al, 2018; Titone, Gullifer, Subramaniapillai, Rajah & Baum, 2017), drive linguistic performance and executive control abilities. However, there is not yet consensus on the best practices for measuring current language experience (Baum & Titone, 2014; Gollan, Weissberger, Runnqvist, Montoya & Cera, 2012; Surrain & Luk, 2017; Takahesu Tabori, Mech & Atagi, 2018; Tomoschuk, Ferreira & Gollan, 2018) despite its theoretical importance (Abutalebi & Green, 2016; Green & Abutalebi, 2013), in large part because bilingualism and bilingual experience are not homogenous constructs.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To investigate whether effects of CMRs and APOE risk varied across different age groups, we categorised participants' ages (i.e., Age Group) using the following bins: 45-55 years, 56-65 years, and 66-82 years (Table 2). The bins were selected to take the full cohort age range into account, and to enable comparisons of effects in middle-age and different stages of older adulthood in line with previous studies examining participants throughout the adult lifespan [80,81,82]. Distributions for BMI, WHR, and BF% within each Age Group are shown in SI Figure 2.…”
Section: Categorising Groups Based On I) Age At Menopause and Ii) Chronological Agementioning
confidence: 99%
“…In this study, we examined the associations between WM BAG and key CMRs, including BMI, WHR, and BF% [77,78,79], and APOE4 status in males (N = 10,605) and females (N = 10,703). We further assessed whether these risk factors had salient effects in middle age (44-55yrs) and different stages of older adulthood (56-65yrs and 66-82yrs) [80,81,82].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Selective survival may be occurring in this subset of women who engage in reserve factors that increase resistance in women more than men. Although our review did not find studies that considered sex differences in all of the reserve factors considered, studies do demonstrate the importance of education (Letteneur et al, 2000, Oliviera et al, 2016, Koran et al, 2017, bilingualism (Subramaniapillai et al, 2018), and physical activity (Baker et al, 2010;Barha et al, 2017;Colcombe and Kramer, 2003;Van Uffelen et al, 2008) to women's resistance to age-related cognitive decline.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 70%