2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2011.07.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Bilingual language learning: An ERP study relating early brain responses to speech, language input, and later word production

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
130
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 230 publications
(136 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
6
130
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, one study that used brain measures of phonetic discrimination (i.e., ERPs) found that Spanish-English DLL infants between 6- and 9-months of age did not show evidence of neural discrimination of Spanish or English contrasts that monolingual infants showed at that age. Evidence of neural discrimination of contrasts did not occur until 10- to 12-months of age in DLLs (Garcia-Sierra et al, 2011). It is thought that this U-shaped pattern may occur because children exposed to two languages require more time to accumulate sufficient data to discriminate the two sets of phonetic categories they must learn.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…For example, one study that used brain measures of phonetic discrimination (i.e., ERPs) found that Spanish-English DLL infants between 6- and 9-months of age did not show evidence of neural discrimination of Spanish or English contrasts that monolingual infants showed at that age. Evidence of neural discrimination of contrasts did not occur until 10- to 12-months of age in DLLs (Garcia-Sierra et al, 2011). It is thought that this U-shaped pattern may occur because children exposed to two languages require more time to accumulate sufficient data to discriminate the two sets of phonetic categories they must learn.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Four of these studies were conducted in United States with infants between 6 and 20 months, all being exposed to Spanish and English (Conboy & Mills, 2006; Conboy & Kuhl, 2011; Garcia-Sierra et al, 2011; Shafer, Yu, & Garrido-Nag, 2012). The remaining six studies were conducted in Japan (Japanese and English; Hidaka et al, 2012; Takahashi et al, 2011), United Kingdom (Welsh and English; Kuipers & Thierry, 2012), Canada (English in addition to French, Spanish, Chinese; Petitto et al, 2012), Germany (German and Turkish; Rinker, Alku, Brosch, & Kiefer, 2010) and Finland (Finnish and French; Shestakova, Huotilainen, Ceponiene, & Cheour, 2003).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One-baby learners showed a positive mismatch response (MMR) that our laboratory and others interpret as increased auditory attention to an acoustic stimulus, but not one associated with phonetic learning (Morr et al 2002). Our data show that positive MMR responses become a more negative with increased exposure to a phonetic contrast (García-Sierra et al 2011). Infants in the two-baby learning condition showed a negative MMR in response to the phonetic contrast, a response that resembles the negative MMR shown in phonetic tests on monolingual adults and infants (Näätänen et al 1997).…”
Section: For Details)mentioning
confidence: 44%
“…Bilingual learners acquire two languages, using the same principles, although our data suggest that the sensitive period for learning is extended (García-Sierra et al 2011). We are currently investigating the extended sensitive period in bilinguals: The neural system may remain "open" longer due to variability in overall language input or due to reduction in input per unit time for each of the native languages, thus extending the learning process (see Kuhl et al 2008, for discussion).…”
Section: The Timing Of Infant Learningmentioning
confidence: 93%