Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior 2017
DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_1301-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Biological Preparedness

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
2
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
1

Relationship

0
1

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 1 publication
(3 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
1
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, (i) ancestral organisms that could learn some cue combinations but not others would eventually give rise to descendants that could learn any cue combination; (ii) ancestral organisms that required multiple exposures to learn the cue-response association gave rise to descendants that required fewer exposures and, ultimately, final organisms that required only a single exposure; and (iii) in environments without cue reversals, ancestral organisms that could re-form associations multiple times gave rise to final organisms that could imprint only once. These adaptations are consistent with the literature on preparedness and other so-called constraints on learning (Seligman 1970;Shettleworth 2010;Domjan 2012;Dunlap 2017) as well as the literature on sensitive periods of plasticity (Bateson 1979;Cashdan 1994;Fawcett and Frankenhuis 2015). The key themes are that evolution produces learning mechanisms that are optimized for the needs of an animal in the environment where it evolved, and since learning is costly, evolution will often restrict the periods of an animal's life when it is most capable of learning (sensitive periods).…”
Section: How Evolution Continues To Shape Associative Learningsupporting
confidence: 82%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…For example, (i) ancestral organisms that could learn some cue combinations but not others would eventually give rise to descendants that could learn any cue combination; (ii) ancestral organisms that required multiple exposures to learn the cue-response association gave rise to descendants that required fewer exposures and, ultimately, final organisms that required only a single exposure; and (iii) in environments without cue reversals, ancestral organisms that could re-form associations multiple times gave rise to final organisms that could imprint only once. These adaptations are consistent with the literature on preparedness and other so-called constraints on learning (Seligman 1970;Shettleworth 2010;Domjan 2012;Dunlap 2017) as well as the literature on sensitive periods of plasticity (Bateson 1979;Cashdan 1994;Fawcett and Frankenhuis 2015). The key themes are that evolution produces learning mechanisms that are optimized for the needs of an animal in the environment where it evolved, and since learning is costly, evolution will often restrict the periods of an animal's life when it is most capable of learning (sensitive periods).…”
Section: How Evolution Continues To Shape Associative Learningsupporting
confidence: 82%
“…In addition, all environments provided organisms with the features thought necessary for learning to evolve: frequent choices of actions (move straight, turn right, or turn left) and cues that change each generation but reliably indicate the best choice within a generation (Dunlap andStephens 2009, 2016;Dunlap et al 2019). However, while these features were present in all environments, they proved insufficient to evolve learning.…”
Section: %-14% (Experiments 1 and 2)mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation