2008
DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.01.005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Blocked goals, persistent action: Implementation intentions engender tenacious goal striving

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
16
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

5
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
1
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The present findings are, however, the first to manipulate the use of justifications and examine the effects on counter‐intentional indulgence. Whereas existing studies that examine the processes that occur between forming an intention and subsequent goal striving have focused on external threats to intention realization such as unexpected situations (DiBonaventura & Chapman, ; Martijn et al ., ) or social recognition (Gollwitzer, Sheeran, Michalski, & Seifert, ), the present studies indicate that threats to intention realization can originate within the person, in the form of justifications for indulgence. Our findings provide insight into a particular type of self‐talk (i.e., giving oneself reasons to indulge) that can jeopardize the effective translation of intentions into action.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 50%
“…The present findings are, however, the first to manipulate the use of justifications and examine the effects on counter‐intentional indulgence. Whereas existing studies that examine the processes that occur between forming an intention and subsequent goal striving have focused on external threats to intention realization such as unexpected situations (DiBonaventura & Chapman, ; Martijn et al ., ) or social recognition (Gollwitzer, Sheeran, Michalski, & Seifert, ), the present studies indicate that threats to intention realization can originate within the person, in the form of justifications for indulgence. Our findings provide insight into a particular type of self‐talk (i.e., giving oneself reasons to indulge) that can jeopardize the effective translation of intentions into action.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 50%
“…This is in line with the initial proposition of Gollwitzer (1999) that the effect of IIs is expected to be sustained over time given the persistence of mental procedures underlying IIs. In this regard, it has been observed that forming IIs not only promotes action initiation, but also engenders greater tenacity of goal pursuit when unexpected barriers are encountered (Martijn et al, 2008). These authors attributed this effect to a decrease in demand on the limited self-regulatory resources needed to overcome difficulties associated with goal striving (Muraven & Baumeister, 2000), essentially due to the shift of the control of an action from a deliberate conscious and effortful control to a more automatic regulation.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The self‐regulatory problem of overcoming self‐defensiveness when having to call a halt to a futile goal striving (e.g., disengaging from a means that turns out to be faulty, even though the means was chosen mindfully; Bobocel & Meyer, 1994) has also been found to benefit from forming implementation intentions (Henderson, Gollwitzer, & Oettingen, 2007). And finally, self‐regulatory resources (Muraven & Baumeister, 2000) are found to stay preserved when goal striving is regulated by implementation intentions, and thus for individuals using implementation intentions, not overextending oneself becomes easier (Webb & Sheeran, 2003; Martijn, Alberts, Sheeran, Peters, Mikolajczak, & de Vries, 2008).…”
Section: What Are Implementation Intentions and How Do They Work?mentioning
confidence: 99%