Visual attention mechanisms are known to select information to process based on current goals, personal relevance, and lowerlevel features. Here we present evidence that human visual attention also includes a high-level category-specialized system that monitors animals in an ongoing manner. Exposed to alternations between complex natural scenes and duplicates with a single change (a change-detection paradigm), subjects are substantially faster and more accurate at detecting changes in animals relative to changes in all tested categories of inanimate objects, even vehicles, which they have been trained for years to monitor for sudden life-or-death changes in trajectory. This animate monitoring bias could not be accounted for by differences in lower-level visual characteristics, how interesting the target objects were, experience, or expertise, implicating mechanisms that evolved to direct attention differentially to objects by virtue of their membership in ancestrally important categories, regardless of their current utility.animacy ͉ category specificity ͉ domain specificity ͉ evolutionary psychology ͉ visual attention V isual attention is an umbrella term for the set of operations that select some portions of a scene, rather than others, for more extensive processing. These operations evolved because some categories of information in the visual environment were likely to be more important or time-sensitive than others for activities that contributed to an organism's survival or reproduction. The selection criteria that direct visual attention can be categorized by their origin: (i) goal-derived: criteria activated volitionally in response to a transient internally represented goal; (ii) ancestrally derived: criteria so generally useful for a species, generation after generation, that natural selection favored mechanisms that cause them to develop in a species-typical manner; and (iii) expertise-derived: criteria extracted during ontogeny by evolved mechanisms specialized for detecting which perceptual cues predict information that enhances task performance.These three types of criteria may also interact; for example, differential experience or temporary goals could calibrate or elaborate ancestrally derived criteria built into the attentional architecture.The ways in which human attention can be affected by goals and expertise have been extensively investigated. Indeed, humans are zoologically unique in the extent to which we evolved to engage in behavior tailored to achieve situation-specific goals as a regular part of our subsistence and sociality (1, 2). Among our foraging ancestors, improvising solutions in response to the distinctive features of situations would have benefited from the existence of goal-driven voluntary attentional mechanisms. As predicted by such a view, otherwise arbitrary but task-relevant objects command more attention than task-irrelevant ones (3), and expertise in a task domain shifts attention to more tasksignificant objects (4), features (5), and locations (6).In contrast, attentio...